1999
DOI: 10.1080/00224499909551993
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Sexual messages on television: Comparing findings from three studies

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
26
0

Year Published

2002
2002
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 52 publications
(28 citation statements)
references
References 7 publications
2
26
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Additionally, the current line-up of dating shows is more risqué and provides the viewer with more provocative material than its predecessors (Pursell, 2002). This is consistent with findings that indicate 57% of televised shows between 7 AM and 11 PM contained sexual dialogue and 12% of television shows depicted sexual behaviors (Kunkel et al, 1999). In the majority of television dating shows, sexual content is a major focus of the show including verbal reference, behaviors, and acts.…”
Section: Dating Shows: Past and Presentsupporting
confidence: 85%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Additionally, the current line-up of dating shows is more risqué and provides the viewer with more provocative material than its predecessors (Pursell, 2002). This is consistent with findings that indicate 57% of televised shows between 7 AM and 11 PM contained sexual dialogue and 12% of television shows depicted sexual behaviors (Kunkel et al, 1999). In the majority of television dating shows, sexual content is a major focus of the show including verbal reference, behaviors, and acts.…”
Section: Dating Shows: Past and Presentsupporting
confidence: 85%
“…Although sexual content is a major part of television (Kunkel et al, 1999;Ward, 1995), references to safe sexual practices are becoming more prevalent (The Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation, 2003). A recent study found that dialogue on reality shows, movies, and talk shows are alluding to safer sexual practices (The Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation, 2003).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Consistent with previous research (Kunkel and Cope 1999), sex talk referred to any verbal reference or dialogue regarding sex or sexual issues. Sex talk was coded as present or absent.…”
Section: Overt Sexuality and Objectification Variablesmentioning
confidence: 80%
“…3 Abstracts were inspected, and reports were excluded if the research (a) was not written in English or German; (b) contained no empirical study or no reporting of original data (e.g., interviews, theoretical considerations on risk taking or reanalysis of previous data; Aitken, Eadie, Leathar, McNeill, & Scott, 1988;Garfield, Chung, & Rathouz, 2003;Kunkel, Cope, & Biely, 1999;Roberts & Foehr, 2008); (d) had no relevant measure of exposure to risk-glorifying media content (e.g., Buchanan & Lev, 1989;Eaton et al, 2006;Unger, Johnson, & Rohrbach, 1995); (e) was conducted with a clinical participant sample (such as people who had previously attempted suicide; Berman, 1988); (f) did not have risk-glorifying media content as its stimulus (e.g., antitobacco advertisements; Lee & Ferguson, 2002;Loukas, Murphy, & Gottlieb, 2008;Moyer-Guse, 2008;Tyler & Cook, 1984); (g) did not principally focus on risk-glorifying media content (such as those on media violence and aggression); (h) did not include riskglorifying pictures, films, music, video games, advertisements, or TV programs (e.g., Griffiths, 2005;Lasorsa & Shoemaker, 1988); (i) did not focus on one of the classic risk-taking domains, such as risky driving, smoking, drug consumption, alcohol consumption, gambling, delinquency, or sexual risk taking (thus excluding those on aggression, suicide, eating behavior, body perception, or skipping school, e.g., Ata, Ludden, & Lally, 2007;Robinson, 1999;Scheel & Westefeld, 1999;Vandewater, Shim, & Caplovitz;; (k) included neuropsychological methods (Brewer-Smyth, 2006;Brewer-Smyth, Burgess, & Shults, 2004); (l) did not provide sufficient statistical data to compute an effect size (or where we received no response from the researchers who were contacted for additional statistical data); (m) did not use risk-glorifying but rather risk-negative media depictions (e.g., antitobacco advertisement); and (n) merely assessed the relationship between media exposure and risk taking with path analytical procedures, because no clearly comparable effect size indicators were provided (e.g., Tickle, Hull, Sargent, Dalton, & Heatherton, 2006;Wyllie, Zhang, & Casswell, 1998).…”
Section: Selection Criteriamentioning
confidence: 99%