2019
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0209376
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Shared and differential default-mode related patterns of activity in an autobiographical, a self-referential and an attentional task

Abstract: The default-mode network (DMN) comprises a set of brain regions that show deactivations during performance of attentionally demanding tasks, but also activation during certain processes including recall of autobiographical memories and processing information about oneself, among others. However, the DMN is not activated in a homogeneous manner during performance of such tasks, so it is not clear to what extent its activation patterns correspond to deactivation patterns seen during attention-demanding tasks. In… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

1
13
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

3
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
1
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The task used in the present study was adapted from the one described in Modinos et al. (2009) and has already been used in healthy subjects by our group (Fuentes-Claramonte et al., 2019) to investigate the processing of information related to the self and another known person, compared to general semantic knowledge. To best isolate the components of interest for our study, this task uses (1) a semantic control condition (matched in perceptual and motor requirements and in statement complexity with the conditions of interest) to separate self- and other-reflection from processing of externally-oriented information, and (2) an ‘other’ condition aimed to compare reflection upon oneself to reflection on other individuals within the participant's environment, for which we selected an ‘other’ personally known by the participant (so responses might be based on previous interactions and not so much on semantic knowledge, and so exposure to this ‘other’ might be more uniform across participants than with public figures), but not a very close ‘other’ to avoid strong emotional investment.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The task used in the present study was adapted from the one described in Modinos et al. (2009) and has already been used in healthy subjects by our group (Fuentes-Claramonte et al., 2019) to investigate the processing of information related to the self and another known person, compared to general semantic knowledge. To best isolate the components of interest for our study, this task uses (1) a semantic control condition (matched in perceptual and motor requirements and in statement complexity with the conditions of interest) to separate self- and other-reflection from processing of externally-oriented information, and (2) an ‘other’ condition aimed to compare reflection upon oneself to reflection on other individuals within the participant's environment, for which we selected an ‘other’ personally known by the participant (so responses might be based on previous interactions and not so much on semantic knowledge, and so exposure to this ‘other’ might be more uniform across participants than with public figures), but not a very close ‘other’ to avoid strong emotional investment.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…the direct comparison between self- and other-reflection). We will use a self/other-reflection task based on previous studies (Modinos et al., 2009; van der Meer et al., 2013) that we have already used successfully in healthy subjects (Fuentes-Claramonte et al., 2019), where we observed significant differences between self- and other-reflection in DMN regions with the specific involvement of the right posterior TPJ (angular gyrus) in other, but not self-reflection. Considering the variability in the tasks used in the previous literature, we have selected this task to best isolate the components of interest for our study.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For a detailed explanation of the ANOVA fMRI design used in this study, see see/fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/GLM. The fMRI analyses were centred at the 2‐back versus baseline contrast because this explicitly evaluates differences between rest and working memory activity, which shows the most pronounced differences according to previous studies (Fuentes‐Claramonte et al, ; Pomarol‐Clotet et al, ).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…MRI acquisitions included a T1 structural image, a resting-state fMRI sequence, and an fMRI acquisition obtained during performance of the n back task (a working memory task). Acquisition parameters as well as a detailed description of the preprocessing steps applied to the images can be found in previous reports: T1 (Salvador et al, 2017b), resting-state fMRI (Salvador et al, 2017a), and the n -back task (Fuentes-Claramonte et al, 2019). Images from all modalities were coregistered to the same standard MNI152 T1 2 mm template.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We used images containing the individual regression coefficients for the two main contrasts of the n -bask task: 1back-vs.-baseline and 2back-vs.-baseline (Gevins and Cutillo, 1993; Fuentes-Claramonte et al, 2019). Finally, from the resting-state fMRI sequences, we derived two brain maps containing complementary information.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%