2019
DOI: 10.1101/824839
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Shared neural representations of cognitive conflict and negative affect in the medial frontal cortex

Abstract: AbstractInfluential theories of medial frontal cortex (MFC) function suggest that the MFC registers cognitive conflict as an aversive signal, but no study directly tested this idea. Instead, recent studies suggested that non-overlapping regions in the MFC process conflict and affect. In this pre-registered human fMRI study, we used multivariate pattern analyses to identify which regions respond similarly to conflict and aversive signals. The results reveal that, of all conflict… Show more

Help me understand this report
View published versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
4
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 55 publications
(90 reference statements)
1
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It is now well-established that cognitive conflict (e.g. incongruent Stroop stimuli, task-switching, etc) trigger negative affect (Braem et al, 2017;Dignath & Eder, 2015;Dreisbach & Fischer, 2012;Fritz & Dreisbach, 2013, 2015Schouppe et al, 2012Schouppe et al, , 2015Vermeylen, Wisniewski, et al, 2019). This result is in line with recent theories suggesting cognitive conflict is registered as an aversive event to drive cognitive control (Botvinick, 2007;Dignath et al, 2020;Dreisbach & Fischer, 2012, 2015Inzlicht et al, 2015;van Steenbergen, 2015).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 83%
“…It is now well-established that cognitive conflict (e.g. incongruent Stroop stimuli, task-switching, etc) trigger negative affect (Braem et al, 2017;Dignath & Eder, 2015;Dreisbach & Fischer, 2012;Fritz & Dreisbach, 2013, 2015Schouppe et al, 2012Schouppe et al, , 2015Vermeylen, Wisniewski, et al, 2019). This result is in line with recent theories suggesting cognitive conflict is registered as an aversive event to drive cognitive control (Botvinick, 2007;Dignath et al, 2020;Dreisbach & Fischer, 2012, 2015Inzlicht et al, 2015;van Steenbergen, 2015).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 83%
“…A compelling possibility is that the cognitive control system uses a similar representational format to coordinate multiple control signals within a task as well (Ebitz et al, 2020;Libby and Buschman, 2021;Rust and Cohen, 2022). A large body of work has shown that cognitive control networks encode multiple task parameters (Flesch et al, 2022;Freund et al, 2021;Jackson et al, 2017Jackson et al, , 2021Kayser et al, 2010b;Vermeylen et al, 2020;Woolgar et al, 2011Woolgar et al, , 2015bWoolgar et al, , 2015a, and 'global' measures of cognitive control like overall difficulty or effort (Freund et al, 2021;Kragel et al, 2018;Smith et al, 2019;Vermeylen et al, 2019). However, little is known about whether different control parameters are encoded independently from one another, which would allow the brain to simultaneously coordinate multiple forms of goaldirected task processing.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In his 2007 update of the conflict monitoring theory, Botvinick proposed that anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) may not only monitor conflict but serve a broader function of monitoring aversive events that drive avoidance learning (Botvinick, 2007). This idea was later supported by empirical data and it is now well established that cognitive conflict (e.g., incongruent Stroop stimuli, task-switching) triggers negative affect (Braem et al, 2017;Dignath & Eder, 2015;Dreisbach & Fischer, 2012;Fritz & Dreisbach, 2013Schouppe et al, 2012Schouppe et al, , 2015Vermeylen, Wisniewski, et al, 2019). For example, using the affective priming technique (Fazio, 2001), Dreisbach and Fischer (2012) showed that participants process negative pictures faster than positive pictures, when presented immediately after incongruent Stroop stimuli (e.g., "RED" printed in blue) versus congruent Stroop stimuli ("RED" printed in red).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%