1994
DOI: 10.1177/001872089403600404
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Shifting Visual Attention in Stereographic Displays: A Time Course Analysis

Abstract: According to a growing number of studies, the stereographic presentation of three-dimensional information improves overall performance relative to displays with only monocular depth cues. Based on studies using real-world, three-dimensional stimuli, however, the efficiency (i.e., speed) of shifting attention between objects located at different depths is impaired relative to that of attention shifts between objects located at the same depth. In the present study we tested whether similar impairments occur when… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

3
17
1

Year Published

1997
1997
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 30 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 18 publications
3
17
1
Order By: Relevance
“…A prior study had shown that the spatial IOR effect is not depth-specific in 3-D space (Theeuwes & Pratt, 2003), a finding that is in agreement with the viewpoint that attention does not operate in depth, or is Bdepth-blind^ (Ghirardelli & Folk, 1996;Iavecchia & Folk, 1995). However, our results showed that spatial IOR was depth-specific when targets appeared in the near depth plane, whereas spatial IOR was not depthspecific when targets appeared in the far depth plane.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…A prior study had shown that the spatial IOR effect is not depth-specific in 3-D space (Theeuwes & Pratt, 2003), a finding that is in agreement with the viewpoint that attention does not operate in depth, or is Bdepth-blind^ (Ghirardelli & Folk, 1996;Iavecchia & Folk, 1995). However, our results showed that spatial IOR was depth-specific when targets appeared in the near depth plane, whereas spatial IOR was not depthspecific when targets appeared in the far depth plane.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…That is, in the absence of distractors that are similar in appearance to the targets and with easily distinguishable targets, narrowly focused attention is not necessary to perform the task, thereby precluding the need to reorient attention between cued and uncued locations in depth (Lavie, 1995;Lavie & Tsal, 1994). Thus, the results of our study provide a resolution to the paradox in the literature as to why some studies have yielded evidence for selective attention in depth (Andersen & Kramer, 1993;He & Nakayama, 1995;Hoffman & Mueller, 1994;Nakayama & Silverman, 1986) whereas others have not (Ghiradelli & Folk, 1996;lavecchia & Folk, 1995;Zimba & Tellinghuisen, 1990). Observers appear to selectively attend to locations in depth whenever they must search for a target among a multitude of distractors (He & Nakayama, 1995;Nakayama & Silverman, 1986), perform a focused attention task in the presence of surrounding distractors (Andersen, 1990;Andersen & Kramer, 1993), attend to objects defined with pre-masks (Hoffman & Mueller, 1994), and, as in the present case, reorient attention, in the presence of distractors, between cued and uncued locations.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 51%
“…It is unclear from the existing perceptual psychology literature whether users of a two layer display will be able to attend to single layer or not. Some researchers argue that attention cannot be preferentially allocated to specific locations in depth [lavecchia & Folk 1994;Ghiradelli & Folk 1996;Theeuwes et al 1998]. Hence, common depth is neither a necessary nor sufficient basis for attentional deployment.…”
Section: Attentionmentioning
confidence: 99%