2010
DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.08.076
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Signal enhancement and suppression during visual–spatial selective attention

Abstract: Selective attention involves the relative enhancement of relevant versus irrelevant stimuli. However, whether this relative enhancement involves primarily enhancement of attended stimuli, or suppression of irrelevant stimuli, remains controversial. Moreover, if both enhancement and suppression are involved, whether they result from a single mechanism or separate mechanisms during attentional control or selection is not known. In two experiments using a spatial cuing paradigm with task-relevant targets and irre… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
48
2
2

Year Published

2012
2012
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 51 publications
(57 citation statements)
references
References 70 publications
5
48
2
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Evidence of distractor suppression has been found in several MOT studies (Flombaum, Scholl, & Pylyshyn, 2008;Pylyshyn, 2006;Pylyshyn, Haladjian, King, & Reilly, 2008; but see Drew, McCollough, Horowitz, & Vogel, 2009) and in other attentional tasks (e.g., Awh, Matsukura, & Serences, 2003;Braithwaite, Humphreys, & Hulleman, 2005;Koshino, 2001;Ogawa, Takeda, & Yagi, 2002). Additionally, several studies using neurophysiological measures have found evidence of both target attentional enhancement and distractor suppression (e.g., Couperus & Mangun, 2010;Hillyard, Vogel, & Luck, 1998;Luck, 1995;Pinsk, Doniger, & Kastner, 2004;Somers, Dale, Seiffert, & Tootell, 1999). Although Bettencourt and Somers conceived the target attentional enhancement and distractor suppression processes as both drawing on a single resource pool, if these processes are characterized as two independent resource pools, then the flexible-resource model would account for the results of the present study.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…Evidence of distractor suppression has been found in several MOT studies (Flombaum, Scholl, & Pylyshyn, 2008;Pylyshyn, 2006;Pylyshyn, Haladjian, King, & Reilly, 2008; but see Drew, McCollough, Horowitz, & Vogel, 2009) and in other attentional tasks (e.g., Awh, Matsukura, & Serences, 2003;Braithwaite, Humphreys, & Hulleman, 2005;Koshino, 2001;Ogawa, Takeda, & Yagi, 2002). Additionally, several studies using neurophysiological measures have found evidence of both target attentional enhancement and distractor suppression (e.g., Couperus & Mangun, 2010;Hillyard, Vogel, & Luck, 1998;Luck, 1995;Pinsk, Doniger, & Kastner, 2004;Somers, Dale, Seiffert, & Tootell, 1999). Although Bettencourt and Somers conceived the target attentional enhancement and distractor suppression processes as both drawing on a single resource pool, if these processes are characterized as two independent resource pools, then the flexible-resource model would account for the results of the present study.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…That is, the vast majority of attention studies reporting increased preparatory alpha activity over irrelevant visual regions used task designs in which foreknowledge about the distractor location or features was accompanied by foreknowledge about the target location or features, such as bilateral displays, rendering it possible that observed effects could also simply reflect attending away or secondary inhibition related to attention to the target . The same is true for studies that used cues that signaled the likelihood of upcoming distraction, but also provided information about the upcoming target, and examined changes in other measures of preparatory activity, such as the cue‐evoked LDAP ERP component …”
Section: Direct Preparatory Distractor Suppressionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…They reported WMC-related differences in early event-related potentials (ERPs) elicited by task-irrelevant probes at to-be-ignored locations, and interpreted such differences as a deficiency of low-WMC individuals to suppress distracting information. However, attention-related changes in early ERPs seem to reflect both suppression of irrelevant information and enhancement of relevant information (Couperus and Mangun, 2012), making it difficult to judge the relative contribution of these two processes in the Fukuda and Vogel (Fukuda and Vogel, 2009) results. Moreover, the WMC scores and electrophysiological indices of effectiveness of attentional control were derived from the tasks that share many features (e.g., multiple-stimulus arrays and short stimulus presentation times), thus raising concerns about the generalizability of the reported effects to other experimental contexts (cf., Mall et al, 2014), and calling for the need to investigate how attentional control is related to WMC using other typical WMC measures such as complex span tasks (Kane et al, 2004;Unsworth et al, 2005).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%