2017
DOI: 10.1080/23311908.2017.1288351
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Social power and dimensions of self-control: Does power benefit initiatory self-control but impair inhibitory self-control?

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Although it is still far from achieving a field-wide consensus (Baggetta and Alexander, 2016 ), attempts to systematically classify and operationally define different facets of EFs (e.g., updating, shifting, and inhibition; Miyake et al, 2000 ) have contributed to developing some initial consensus, which has helped researchers judge whether a task implicates EF processes. Analogous attempts would be helpful for self-control research, especially if such efforts can help systematically examine which facets of self-control are linked to the ego-depletion phenomenon (e.g., Fujita, 2011 ; Heller et al, 2017 ).…”
Section: Lack Of Clear Operational Definitions Of Self-controlmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although it is still far from achieving a field-wide consensus (Baggetta and Alexander, 2016 ), attempts to systematically classify and operationally define different facets of EFs (e.g., updating, shifting, and inhibition; Miyake et al, 2000 ) have contributed to developing some initial consensus, which has helped researchers judge whether a task implicates EF processes. Analogous attempts would be helpful for self-control research, especially if such efforts can help systematically examine which facets of self-control are linked to the ego-depletion phenomenon (e.g., Fujita, 2011 ; Heller et al, 2017 ).…”
Section: Lack Of Clear Operational Definitions Of Self-controlmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…They are more likely to act on internal driving forces (e.g., their own values and attitudes) and are less affected by external cues (especially peers) [25]. High-power individuals tend to show better self-control than low-power individuals [26]. Because they have more advantages when balancing goals and adjusting their attention [27], they control their impulsiveness or adopt delayed gratification strategies to achieve long-term goals.…”
Section: Sense Of Power As a Moderatormentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Because they have more advantages when balancing goals and adjusting their attention [27], they control their impulsiveness or adopt delayed gratification strategies to achieve long-term goals. Heller et al [26] showed that when a task requires the participation of self-control, a high sense of power can increase self-control to sufficiently initiate and maintain behavior, thereby promoting the completion of the task.…”
Section: Sense Of Power As a Moderatormentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Following this rationale, each transfer level can be matched with an increasingly dissimilar task. In our case, this includes (1) an auditory SST, which differs only in terms of response modality (modality transfer), (2) a Stroop task, which differs only in terms of the task-related operation (near transfer), (3) a flanker task, which differs both in terms of the task-related operation and the underlying inhibitory function (far transfer), and (4) an unsolvable anagram task, in which the contribution of inhibitory processes should be minor relative to initiatory processes that boost persistence and problem-solving (Allom & Mullan, 2014;Heller et al, 2017).…”
Section: A Taxonomy Of Self-control Tasksmentioning
confidence: 99%