Handbook of Behavior Genetics 2009
DOI: 10.1007/978-0-387-76727-7_20
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Some Guidelines for Defining Personality Differences in Rats

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

3
25
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 23 publications
(28 citation statements)
references
References 208 publications
3
25
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It is noteworthy in this context that previous studies have shown that: (a) immobility counts of RLA rats are comparable to those of unselected Wistar rats, and (b) RLA rats are more responsive than unselected rats to the immobility-reducing effect of low doses of antidepressant drugs (our unpublished results). However, those results should be interpreted with caution because it has been shown that even subtle variations in housing conditions and laboratory environment can alter rodent anxiety-and depression-related behavior (Crabbe et al 1999;Driscoll et al 2009;Escorihuela et al 1995b). Therefore, it should be taken into account that, to address this issue, we cannot simply compare the behavior of RHA and RLA rats bred in our laboratory with that of commercially available rats which have been subjected to different pre-and post-natal housing conditions, since these breeding differences may affect the outcome of behavioral experiments.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…It is noteworthy in this context that previous studies have shown that: (a) immobility counts of RLA rats are comparable to those of unselected Wistar rats, and (b) RLA rats are more responsive than unselected rats to the immobility-reducing effect of low doses of antidepressant drugs (our unpublished results). However, those results should be interpreted with caution because it has been shown that even subtle variations in housing conditions and laboratory environment can alter rodent anxiety-and depression-related behavior (Crabbe et al 1999;Driscoll et al 2009;Escorihuela et al 1995b). Therefore, it should be taken into account that, to address this issue, we cannot simply compare the behavior of RHA and RLA rats bred in our laboratory with that of commercially available rats which have been subjected to different pre-and post-natal housing conditions, since these breeding differences may affect the outcome of behavioral experiments.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…However, the concordant lack of changes in accuracy and the increased response latency in RLA-I rats point to motivational/ emotional factors (rather than impairments in attentional processes) as a possible mechanism underlying strain differences for this parameter. Compared with RHA-I rats, RLA-I rats are characterized by increased anxiety/fear levels, enhanced (hormonal and behavioral) sensitivity to stressful situations, and shows of characteristic passive coping strategies (eg, freezing behavior) when faced with conflict or stressful situations (Carrasco et al, 2008;Driscoll et al, 2009;López-Aumatell et al, 2009a, b;Driscoll, 2003, 2005). In this sense, it appears likely that the demanding (ie, stressful) conditions of the 5-CSRT task could have influenced motivational/emotional aspects in RLA-I rats, possibly leading to their longer response latencies and increased errors of omissions.…”
Section: Increased Adjunctive Drinking Acquisition In Rha-i Ratsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Two phenotypes with different emotional and motivational profiles have been developed through bidirectional selection and breeding of the Roman high-(RHA) and low-avoidance (RLA) rat lines/ strains (outbred or inbred, respectively), selected for rapid (RHA) vs extremely poor (RLA) acquisition of two-way active avoidance in the shuttle-box (Broadhurst and Bignami, 1965;Driscoll and Bättig, 1982) (for reviews, see Driscoll et al, 1990Driscoll et al, , 1998Driscoll et al, , 2009Escorihuela et al, 1995Escorihuela et al, , 1999Fernández-Teruel et al, 1997;Giorgi et al, 2007;Steimer and Driscoll, 2005). These lines/strains show consistent differences in sensation/novelty seeking and in stress/anxiety endocrine and behavioral responses when confronted with novel environments (ie, intended to measure anxiety, emotionality, fearfulness, or novelty seeking), such as the open-field test (Aubry et al, 1995;Carrasco et al, 2008;Escorihuela et al, 1999;Gentsch et al, 1991), elevated plus maze (Escorihuela et al, 1999;Steimer and Driscoll, 2003), elevated zero-maze (López-Aumatell et al, 2009b), blackwhite box test, dark-light open-field test, dark-light hexagonal tunnel maze (Fernández-Teruel et al, 2002b;Steimer and Driscoll, 2005), and hole-board test (Escorihuela et al, 1999;Fernández-Teruel et al, 1992).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The original psychogenetic selection procedure (based on good vs. extremely poor acquisition of the two-way active avoidance response) has led to stable strain divergence related to anxiety/fearfulness and behavioral inhibition/activation traits (i.e. coping style; [29]). Consistent with these results, Roman rats showed significant differences in frustration paradigms such as instrumental and consummatory successive negative contrast effect [30][31][32], extinction [33,34], partial reinforcement extinction effect [35], and partial reinforcement contrast effect [36].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%