1986
DOI: 10.1113/jphysiol.1986.sp015981
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Spatial and temporal limits of vision in the achromat.

Abstract: SUMMARY1. Threshold detection for sine-wave grating stimuli of varying spatial and temporal frequency was used to investigate the nature of spatial and temporal post-receptoral sensitivity in the typical, complete achromat.2. Threshold spatial and temporal sensitivities under low photopic conditions show no evidence of cone function. The abrupt fall-off in sensitivity is consistent with known psychophysical and electrophysiological measures of rod saturation.3. Threshold spatial and temporal sensitivities unde… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
56
0

Year Published

1990
1990
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
4
4

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 107 publications
(62 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
6
56
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The CSF plotted on a log-log scale has the typical inverted-U shape with a fall-out of sensitivity both at low and at high spatial frequencies (compare with Robson, 1966, at a mean luminance of 20 cd/m 2 , temporal frequency of 1 Hz, and fixed field size). Both observers have normal (extrapolated) visual acuity given the mean luminance level used (Hess & Nordby, 1986, their Figure 6, subject with normal vision).…”
Section: Fitting the Model To Datamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The CSF plotted on a log-log scale has the typical inverted-U shape with a fall-out of sensitivity both at low and at high spatial frequencies (compare with Robson, 1966, at a mean luminance of 20 cd/m 2 , temporal frequency of 1 Hz, and fixed field size). Both observers have normal (extrapolated) visual acuity given the mean luminance level used (Hess & Nordby, 1986, their Figure 6, subject with normal vision).…”
Section: Fitting the Model To Datamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The source of the relationship between sensitivity to sinewave gratings and retinal illuminance seems to require a new theoretical justification. Hess and Nordby (1986b) and Figure 6 in Hess and Nordby (1986a) were digitally scanned at a resolution of 600 dots per inch and the digital versions of these figures were used to extract the coordinates of the data points plotted on them. The empirical data in Figure 1 of Hess and Nordby (1986b) were described by the function CSF(x) = 188 x 0.82 exp [-0.25 …”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These curves represent fits to data from Figure 1 of Hess and Nordby (1986b) and Figure 6 of Hess and Nordby (1986a), which come from the same normal subject. The empirical CSF was obtained at a reportedly optimal illuminance of 2000 scotopic td (optimal in that sensitivity did not increase significantly at higher illuminance levels) and the empirical acuity limit was obtained by determining the highest frequency that could be resolved at a 100% contrast at each illuminance.…”
Section: Sensitivity As a Function Of Spatial Frequency And Illuminancementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…TDepartment of Psychology, University of California at San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093-0109, U.S.A. Nordby, 1986a) and scotopic flicker threshold vs intensity (TVI) profiles measured with frequencies > 8 Hz (Conner, 1982;. At or near 15 Hz, there is a suprathreshold intensity region, adjoining the double-branched flicker TV1 function, within which the perception of flicker completely disappears (see Fig.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%