1980
DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(80)90110-8
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Spatial frequency tuning studies: Weighting as a prerequisite for describing psychometric curves by probability summation

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

1980
1980
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, they do not show the Form one would expect based on the ~sum~tion of tuning, as measured by Bryngdaht (1966), and as needed to describe the visibility of gratings in an equal luminance surround. as a function of the width of the grating (for a further discussion on this see Kroon and van der Wildt, 1980). In our opinion, however, the results of Figs 2 and 3 do not necessarily preclude the existence of tuning.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 46%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…However, they do not show the Form one would expect based on the ~sum~tion of tuning, as measured by Bryngdaht (1966), and as needed to describe the visibility of gratings in an equal luminance surround. as a function of the width of the grating (for a further discussion on this see Kroon and van der Wildt, 1980). In our opinion, however, the results of Figs 2 and 3 do not necessarily preclude the existence of tuning.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 46%
“…This effect can be eliminated by using two-dimensiona gratings. The results of a study which used such twodimensional gratings were described elsewhere (Rijsdijk, Kroon and van der Wildt (1980).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Importantly, visual performance varies not only across eccentricity, but also as a function of polar angle ( Figure 1 ); at matched eccentricity, performance is better along the horizontal than the vertical meridian (horizontal-vertical asymmetry, or ‘anisotropy’; HVA) and better along the lower than the upper vertical meridian (vertical meridian asymmetry; VMA) ( Carrasco et al, 2001 ; Cameron et al, 2002 ; Montaser-Kouhsari and Carrasco, 2009 ; Corbett and Carrasco, 2011 ; Abrams et al, 2012 ; Baldwin et al, 2012 ; Greenwood et al, 2017 ; Himmelberg et al, 2020 ; Barbot et al, 2021 ). These asymmetries are present in several basic perceptual dimensions, including contrast sensitivity ( Rovamo and Virsu, 1979 ; Kroon and van der Wildt, 1980 ; Robson and Graham, 1981 ; Regan and Beverley, 1983 ; Pointer and Hess, 1989 ; Carrasco et al, 2001 ; Cameron et al, 2002 ; Fuller et al, 2008 ; Corbett and Carrasco, 2011 ; Abrams et al, 2012 ; Baldwin et al, 2012 ; Rosén et al, 2014 ; Himmelberg et al, 2020 ; Purokayastha et al, 2021 ) and spatial resolution ( Anderson et al, 1992 ; Mackeben, 1999 ; Altpeter et al, 2000 ; Carrasco et al, 2002 ; Talgar and Carrasco, 2002 ; Montaser-Kouhsari and Carrasco, 2009 ; Wilkinson et al, 2016 ; Greenwood et al, 2017 ; Barbot et al, 2021 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This effect of number of cycles in the stimulus has been attributed to probability summation across a population of independent detectors that are spatially distributed in the visual field. 4,9,10 Improvements in contrast sensitivity achieved by increasing the number of cycles in the stimulus yields a corresponding increase in spatial resolution for foveal vision. 1,12 This is to be expected if we conceive of resolution acuity as that spatial frequency for which contrast sensitivity falls to unity.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%