2011
DOI: 10.1080/0163853x.2010.549452
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Spatial Strategies in the Description of Complex Configurations

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
24
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 38 publications
(25 citation statements)
references
References 40 publications
1
24
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This is based on preferences that are systematically related to egocentric vs. allocentric perspectives, though these, as such, may remain inaccessible to conscious reflection. Moreover, numerous studies show that reference systems are consistently (though not explicitly) expressed in language and can therefore be derived from how descriptions are formulated, even though potential ambiguity may complicate the analysis and necessitate a direct comparison with the observed scene (Carlson, 1999;Carlson-Radvansky & Logan, 1997;Taylor & Tversky, 1996;Tenbrink, Coventry, & Andonova, 2011).…”
Section: The Tunnel Task Paradigmmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is based on preferences that are systematically related to egocentric vs. allocentric perspectives, though these, as such, may remain inaccessible to conscious reflection. Moreover, numerous studies show that reference systems are consistently (though not explicitly) expressed in language and can therefore be derived from how descriptions are formulated, even though potential ambiguity may complicate the analysis and necessitate a direct comparison with the observed scene (Carlson, 1999;Carlson-Radvansky & Logan, 1997;Taylor & Tversky, 1996;Tenbrink, Coventry, & Andonova, 2011).…”
Section: The Tunnel Task Paradigmmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The distinction has been made by several psychologists and linguists (Miller & Johnson-Laird, 1972;Talmy, 1983;Landau & Jackendoff, 1993). The common idea of all these theories is that a spatial relation refers to the position of a particular object in focus relative to another object or area (Tenbrink, Andonova, & Coventry, 2011).…”
Section: Kiosk -I -Ice Cartmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In many cases, specifi c lexical items that have been identifi ed by cognitive linguists (e.g., Talmy, 2000) as refl ecting cognitive structure may be indicative of underlying conceptual patterns. For example, explorations of the chosen level of detail (Daniel & Denis, 2004;Vorwerg & Tenbrink, 2007), hierarchical description levels (Plumert, Carswell, de Vet, & Ihrig, 1995), and the underlying conceptual perspective (Tenbrink, Coventry, & Andonova, 2011;Tenbrink, Ross, Thomas, Dethlefs, & Andonova, 2010;Tversky, 1999) reveal the fl exibility of these concepts relative to changes in the task scenario. For example, Plumert et al ( 1995 ) showed how the discourse task as well as the spatial confi guration of landmarks aff ected the order and hierarchical structuring of spatial descriptions.…”
Section: G a Is At The Top Left B Is Next To A And C Below B )mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, content analysis provides a fi rst basis for categorizing the data with respect to each segment's relation to the discourse task (related to the research question at hand). For instance, in Tenbrink, Coventry, and Andonova (2011), utterances were categorized as to whether they described an object's location or orientation, both, or neither. These categories were identifi ed because they emerged as prominent types of content produced by the speakers, with clear eff ects on the targets of the research design.…”
Section: Scenario Variationmentioning
confidence: 99%