2020
DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2020.147064
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Spatiotemporal brain signal associated with high and low levels of proactive motor response inhibition

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 60 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…There was evidence of proactive slowing from session one to session two, reflected in the delayed going response. This slowing is purported to be an example of proactive motor RI (Brevers et al 2020 ; Greenhouse and Wessel 2013 ; Leotti and Wager 2010 ; Schachar et al 2004 ; Verbruggen et al 2013 , 2008 ) and could be attributed to participants focusing on successful stopping at the expense of fast responses on Go trials (i.e. the opposite strategy to participants in Enge et al 2014 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There was evidence of proactive slowing from session one to session two, reflected in the delayed going response. This slowing is purported to be an example of proactive motor RI (Brevers et al 2020 ; Greenhouse and Wessel 2013 ; Leotti and Wager 2010 ; Schachar et al 2004 ; Verbruggen et al 2013 , 2008 ) and could be attributed to participants focusing on successful stopping at the expense of fast responses on Go trials (i.e. the opposite strategy to participants in Enge et al 2014 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There was evidence of proactive slowing from session one to session two, reflected in the delayed going response. This slowing is purported to be an example of proactive motor RI (Schachar et al, 2004; Verbruggen et al, 2008; Verbruggen et al, 2013; Brevers et al, 2020)and could be attributed to participants focusing on successful stopping at the expense of fast responses on Go trials (i.e. the opposite strategy to participants in Enge et al 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In previous single-task studies, the medial frontal slow negative potential identified as CNV ( Walter et al, 1964 ) has been consistently employed as a marker of the amount of cognitive effort devoted to preparation and pro-active task control (e.g., Tecce, 1972 ; van Boxtel and Brunia, 1994 ; Chaillou et al, 2018 ; Gajewski et al, 2020b ; Thönes et al, 2018 ), including proactive inhibitory control ( Liebrand et al, 2017 ; Brevers et al, 2020 ) and temporal orienting ( Berchicci et al, 2020 ). According to current regression models, enhanced midline fronto-central negative potentials predicted improved performance of both T1 and T2 ( Figure 4 and Supplementary Table 3 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%