2016
DOI: 10.1007/s10683-016-9477-0
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Status quo effects in fairness games: reciprocal responses to acts of commission versus acts of omission

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

2
19
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

3
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 40 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
2
19
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Our results can also be explained by a model of revealed altruism42,43, according to which initial resource allocation affects perceptions of generosity of actions and hence subsequent reciprocity. Because in Provision cooperation is the result of an act of commission (contributing), while in Maintenance cooperation is achieved by omission (not withdrawing), cooperation in Provision is perceived as more generous than in Maintenance and thus Provision triggers stronger positive reciprocity than Maintenance.…”
mentioning
confidence: 52%
“…Our results can also be explained by a model of revealed altruism42,43, according to which initial resource allocation affects perceptions of generosity of actions and hence subsequent reciprocity. Because in Provision cooperation is the result of an act of commission (contributing), while in Maintenance cooperation is achieved by omission (not withdrawing), cooperation in Provision is perceived as more generous than in Maintenance and thus Provision triggers stronger positive reciprocity than Maintenance.…”
mentioning
confidence: 52%
“…For each (X,Y) dilemma, action is more likely in the INDIRECT Frame Secondly, both individuals and courts of law consider an act of omission to be a lesser "sin" than an act of commission that results in similar consequences (see Cox et al, 2017). This principle with respect to the Trolley dilemma has been labelled the "action principle".…”
Section: H1 (Trolley)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Nevertheless, others have found it useful for studying various components of moral reasoning (e.g., Cushman et al, 2006;Greene et al, 2001;Greene et al, 2011), such as the identification of behavioural norms or highlighting that certain moral dilemmas preferentially engage emotional centers in a way that may be important in predicting choice (e.g., Greene et al, 2001). Still others have noted how the Trolley dilemma can highlight the difference between acts of omission versus commission (Spranca et al, 1991;Cox et al, 2017), which is a relevant distinction in courts of law. And, while past criticism of the Trolley dilemma may have seemed justified due to the unrealistic nature of the decision it presents, the relevance of the Trolley dilemma is at a higher level than perhaps ever before with the recent rise in ethical concerns surrounding self-driving vehicles.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…SeeDufwenberg and Gneezy (2000) andCox et al (2010) for similar results in a slightly different context. 7 A look at the distribution of contributions conditional on each continuation probability shows that it is not unimodal-which supports the use of a mixture model.…”
mentioning
confidence: 68%