2019
DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2019.101223
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Successfully learning non-adjacent dependencies in a continuous artificial language stream

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

6
32
1

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(39 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
6
32
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Of the remaining six clips, three (X 1–3 ) occurred exhaustively in the middle position of a_b , and the other three (X 4–6 ) occurred exhaustively in the middle position of c_d (creating a total of six unique triplets). Prior studies have shown that three intervening items provides sufficient variability for adults to learn the NADs in speech [ 30 , 31 ] and in action sequences similar to these [ 42 ]. The assignment from action clips to the letters here were randomized for each infant, so that unintended similarities between the non-adjacent items—for example, actions both involve the hand—would not be confounded with the NADs.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Of the remaining six clips, three (X 1–3 ) occurred exhaustively in the middle position of a_b , and the other three (X 4–6 ) occurred exhaustively in the middle position of c_d (creating a total of six unique triplets). Prior studies have shown that three intervening items provides sufficient variability for adults to learn the NADs in speech [ 30 , 31 ] and in action sequences similar to these [ 42 ]. The assignment from action clips to the letters here were randomized for each infant, so that unintended similarities between the non-adjacent items—for example, actions both involve the hand—would not be confounded with the NADs.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some researchers even theorize that humans are constrained to learn NADs only when the non-adjacent items are at the edges of sequences, as defined by brief silences [ 29 ]. Importantly though, other types of edge or boundary cues appear to facilitate NAD learning, such as top-down structural cues [ 30 ] and rhythmic cues [ 31 ]. Indeed, in those studies [ 30 , 31 ], adults learned NADs without silences at the edges of the NADs, and with minimal variability between only three items in the middle position.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Comparable to infant studies, behavioral studies in adults have also revealed successful NAD learning when adults were tested under the same passive listening conditions ( Frost and Monaghan, 2016 ; Gómez, 2002 ; Onnis et al, 2004 ; Wang et al, 2019 ). The majority of studies in adults, however, involves metalinguistic components from active listening paradigms, with either a 2AFC task (e.g., Frost and Monaghan, 2016 ), a familiarity judgment task ( Wang et al, 2019 ), or a grammatical judgment task (e.g., Gómez, 2002 ; Onnis et al, 2004 ). Thus, it remains unclear whether behavioral studies assessing NAD learning in adults tap into the same learning mechanisms as infant behavioral NAD learning studies.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Children learned to identify words in the bisyllabic language, but not the language with varying lengths. Similarly, Wang, Zevin, and Mintz [52] proposed that early stages of language learning are only possible if the structure to be acquired is regular and rhythmical-if word length varies, then Wang et al [52] predict that learning will not be successful. Thus, prior findings offer a mixed account of whether infants can indeed segment words of varying lengths from speech using statistical regularities alone.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%