Oxford Handbooks Online 2014
DOI: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199686858.013.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Tactile and Haptic Perceptual Organization

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
1

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 86 publications
0
3
1
Order By: Relevance
“…One possibility is that this was caused by the principles of perceptual organization. For instance, similarity of textures between an object and the background structure affects the perception of a single object (as opposed to just the background structure), indicating a grouping principle of similarity [23], [30], [31]. However, in the present study, the compliance of the hard texture was more similar to that of the bars than that of the soft textures.…”
Section: The Effect Of Texturescontrasting
confidence: 68%
“…One possibility is that this was caused by the principles of perceptual organization. For instance, similarity of textures between an object and the background structure affects the perception of a single object (as opposed to just the background structure), indicating a grouping principle of similarity [23], [30], [31]. However, in the present study, the compliance of the hard texture was more similar to that of the bars than that of the soft textures.…”
Section: The Effect Of Texturescontrasting
confidence: 68%
“…In vision, saturation affects the perception of hue, but variation in shape does not affect the perception of color (Burns & Shepp, 1988; Garner, 1976). In haptics, size, shape, orientation, and weight all interact with each other (Kappers & Tiest, 2015). This distinction between separable and integral dimensions has been studied using various approaches, such as similarity measurements, classification tasks, preference judgments, and discriminability between preferences (Garner, 1974, p. 118).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Tactile sensation, typically associated with the sensation of pressure, orientation, curvature, texture, thermal properties, puncture, and vibration [29], is perceived primarily through stimulation of the skin [30] where cutaneous receptors (mechanoreceptors and thermoreceptors) are located [31,32]. Kinesthetic sensation, associated with body position and movement, is perceived through stimulation to the kinesthetic receptors located in muscles, joints, and tendons [29,33,34]. Therefore, the term “tactile”, mediated only through cutaneous receptors, can be considered as a sub-category of “haptic”.…”
Section: A Sense Of Touchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Cutaneous receptors, located across the entire surface of the body (beneath both hairy and hairless parts), include mechanoreceptors (responsible for perceptions of pressure, slip, and vibration), thermoreceptors (for temperature perception), and nociceptors (for pain perception) [32]. There are four main types of mechanoreceptors: (1) slowly-adapting (SA) type I receptors (SA I; small receptive field) that end in Merkel cells, (2) slowly-adapting type II receptors (SA II; large receptive field) that end in Ruffling corpuscles, (3) fast-adapting (FA) type I afferents (FA I; small receptive field) that end in Meissner corpuscles, and (4) fast-adapting (FA) type II afferents (FA II; large receptive field) that end in Pacinian corpuscles [32,33]. The responses of these receptors to stimuli are dependent on two factors: (1) the receptive field size (i.e., the skin region in which the neurons can detect relevant signals) and (2) the relative adaptation rate (i.e., the rate at which the neurons adapt to a constant or static stimulus applied to the skin) [32].…”
Section: A Sense Of Touchmentioning
confidence: 99%