1986
DOI: 10.1016/0001-6918(86)90066-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Target-noise separation in visual selective attention

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

9
37
0

Year Published

1988
1988
2009
2009

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 50 publications
(46 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
9
37
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In our second experiment, we showed that this reduction in interference is also obtained when the target is spatially repositioned shortly after stimulus onset (a little above or below the starting position). The duration account cannot explain this, because the target remains present within the attended small spatial area (Gatti & Egeth, 1978;Hagenaar & van der Heijden, 1986;Merikle & Gorewich, 1979). In contrast, the results are in accordance with the predictions from the Gestalt account: When the target and the distractor are separated, they are no longer perceived as one entity, and the distractor input is not automatically processed together with the target information.…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 69%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In our second experiment, we showed that this reduction in interference is also obtained when the target is spatially repositioned shortly after stimulus onset (a little above or below the starting position). The duration account cannot explain this, because the target remains present within the attended small spatial area (Gatti & Egeth, 1978;Hagenaar & van der Heijden, 1986;Merikle & Gorewich, 1979). In contrast, the results are in accordance with the predictions from the Gestalt account: When the target and the distractor are separated, they are no longer perceived as one entity, and the distractor input is not automatically processed together with the target information.…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 69%
“…This was done in Experiment 2 by letting the color bar and the distractor word move away from each other. To ensure that both components stayed in the same attention area, the movement was restricted to a very small (<2º) visual angle (Gatti & Egeth, 1978;Hagenaar & van der Heijden, 1986;Wühr & Waszak, 2003).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Consistent with this interpretation, Merikle and Gorewich (1979) observed no decrease of Stroop interference with increasing distance (from 0.5º to 2.5º) when letter size was increased to compensate for acuity loss. Similarly, Hagenaar and Van der Heijden (1986) observed almost identical Stroop effects when a color patch and a word were presented close (1.1º) or far (1.9º) and concluded that spatial attention does not affect processing of Stroop words.…”
mentioning
confidence: 89%
“…In summary, the reviewed studies obtained very little evidence for the idea that spatial attention can modulate the Stroop effect. As long as the distance between a relevant color patch and an irrelevant color word is less than 5º, distance variations do not seem to affect the Stroop effect (e.g., Hagenaar & Van der Heijden, 1986). Only when the distance exceeds 5º can increasing distance reduce, but not eliminate, the Stroop effect (Brown et al, 2002).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Flanking effects, especially in peripheral vision when the distractor information is very close, are attributed to insufficient resolution due to receptor spacing or retino-cortical transmission pooling. Disturbances still occur in peripheral vision when the separation is increased (Bouma, 1970;Hagenaar & van der Heijden, 1986;Miller, 1991;Toet & Levi, 1992). They also persist with increasing separation in parafoveal and foveal vision, although these effects are more restricted for central than for peripheral vision (Leat, Li, & Epp, 1999).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%