1995
DOI: 10.1037/0021-843x.104.2.251
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Task difficulty and cognitive deficits in schizophrenia.

Abstract: Investigators of schizophrenic cognition often produce 2 or more tasks of differing difficulty levels by manipulating a variable that affects the accuracy of both normal and schizophrenic individuals; the investigators find that the variable also affects the difference between the groups in accuracy and conclude that the variable taps a schizophrenic differential deficit. An alternative hypothesis is that task differences in true-score variance artifactually produce the finding. For free-response tasks, group … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
51
0

Year Published

1997
1997
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7
2
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 63 publications
(52 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
(32 reference statements)
1
51
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, this approach has been criticized on a number of grounds, including statistical (e.g., regression-to-the-mean effects; Chapman & Chapman, 1973) and subject-characteristic issues (e.g., systematic unmatching on unknown variables; Meehl, 1971). Although not entirely optimal (see, e.g., Chapman & Chapman, 1989;Miller, Chapman, Chapman, & Collins, 1995), the possibility of a generalized deficit was examined by following the suggestions of Chapman et al (1994) to determine whether patients perform differently on two tasks from what would be predicted on the basis of general performance level.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, this approach has been criticized on a number of grounds, including statistical (e.g., regression-to-the-mean effects; Chapman & Chapman, 1973) and subject-characteristic issues (e.g., systematic unmatching on unknown variables; Meehl, 1971). Although not entirely optimal (see, e.g., Chapman & Chapman, 1989;Miller, Chapman, Chapman, & Collins, 1995), the possibility of a generalized deficit was examined by following the suggestions of Chapman et al (1994) to determine whether patients perform differently on two tasks from what would be predicted on the basis of general performance level.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thomas and Nelson (2001) used proportional change in RTs, and Thomas and colleagues (2004) used the diVerence between z scores on random trials and z scores on sequence trials. Interpretation of diVerence scores between groups on experimental versus control conditions can be problematic when there are baseline diVerences among groups (Chapman, Chapman, Curran, & Miller, 1994;Faust, Balota, Spieler, & Ferraro, 1999;Knight & Silverstein, 2001;MacDonald & Carter, 2002;Miller, Chapman, Chapman, & Collins, 1995;Salthouse & Hedden, 2002). DiVerent ways of handling baseline diVerences can result in diVerent interpretations.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Furthermore, simply focusing on interactions, for example, group (autistic and control) × condition (gender and individual) with RT as the dependent measure may be subject to scaling problems that result merely from differences in baseline performance between the groups rather than from a real interaction between group and the conditions of interest. These analytical problems are well-recognized and have plagued the literature previously, for example, in the domain of schizophrenia (Miller, Chapman, Chapman, & Collins, 1995). In an attempt to be cognizant of these potential scaling problems, we not only use the median (to offset the influence of outliers) for each cell for each individual but also more importantly, the percentage change across the experimental conditions is calculated and compared across groups to control for differences in the absolute base RT.…”
Section: Design and Proceduresmentioning
confidence: 99%