“…Since the introduction of the notion of a prototype into the categorization literature by Rosch and her colleagues (e.g., Mervis & Rosch, 1981; Rosch, 1983), the basic idea has been applied to a wide range of linguistic contexts, including lexical semantics (Lakoff, 1987); tense‐aspect marking (Andersen & Shirai, 1996; Shirai & Andersen, 1995); relative clauses (Diessel & Tomasello, 2005); questions with long‐distance dependencies (Dąbrowska, Rowland, & Theakston, 2009); subject auxiliary inversion (Goldberg, 2006; see also Lakoff & Brugman, 1987; Lambrecht, 1994); and the lexical reorganization that leads to semantic overgeneralization and recovery from overgeneralization, as modeled by an unsupervised neural network (Schyns, 1991). There is also cross‐linguistic evidence demonstrating the role of prototypicality in young children’s acquisition of linguistic constructions (see review by Ibbotson & Tomasello, 2009) as well as in non‐linguistic categories (Boswell & Green, 1982; Ford, 2003; Lasky, 1974). Here we use prototype theory in a novel way: to investigate the semantics of a linguistic construction at two different points in development.…”