2002
DOI: 10.1016/s0079-6123(02)40056-8
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The allocation of attention during smooth pursuit eye movements

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

12
64
2

Year Published

2004
2004
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 79 publications
(78 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
12
64
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Studies using a reaction time (RT) task in which a target was presented along the pursuit trajectory have suggested that, during smooth pursuit, attention moves ahead of the pursued object along the trajectory (Tanaka, Yoshida, & Fukushima, 1998;van Donkelaar, 1999;van Donkelaar & Drew, 2002). For instance, van Donkelaar and Drew had participants view a display containing a central cross ("×") and eight peripheral circles, four placed horizontally on the right side of the cross, and four on the left.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Studies using a reaction time (RT) task in which a target was presented along the pursuit trajectory have suggested that, during smooth pursuit, attention moves ahead of the pursued object along the trajectory (Tanaka, Yoshida, & Fukushima, 1998;van Donkelaar, 1999;van Donkelaar & Drew, 2002). For instance, van Donkelaar and Drew had participants view a display containing a central cross ("×") and eight peripheral circles, four placed horizontally on the right side of the cross, and four on the left.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…They argued that previous findings of faster RTs for a target ahead of the pursuit stimulus than for a target behind it (e.g., van Donkelaar & Drew, 2002) reflected a spatial shift of attention triggered by the abrupt onset of a target (Theeuwes, Kramer, Hahn, & Irwin, 1998;Yantis & Jonides, 1990) rather than the steadystate allocation of attention during pursuit. To investigate the spatial allocation of attention during pursuit, Lovejoy et al used a non-RT task that minimizes abrupt onsets (Deubel & Schneider, 1996).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, Van Donkelaar & Drew (2002) have demonstrated that reaction times to changes in stimuli are reduced if they occur 1-2 degrees in front of the actual pursuit target, but increased if they occur 1-2 degrees behind, suggesting that allocation of attention is biased to a position slightly in front of the target during pursuit. Chen & Holzman (2002) found that performing a brief secondary task (judging which of two horizontal gratings presented above and below the pursuit target has the higher spatial frequency) at the same time or 450 ms after pursuit onset led to impairments in the maintenance of pursuit.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The extra 10 ms difference might be due to biomechanics, or switching of attention from one hemisphere to other. Human psycho-physical data (Van Donkelaar & Drew, 2002) show that attention during pursuit is either on the pursued target or ahead of it. So, for a forward saccade, the attentional resources of the same hemisphere are needed.…”
Section: Results: Model Of Simulations Of Sac-spem Datamentioning
confidence: 99%