Two experiments were directed at distinguishing associative and similarity-based accounts of systematic differences in categorization time for different items in natural categories. Experiment 1 investigated the correlation of categorization time with three measures of instance centrality in a category. Production frequency (PF), rated typicality, and familiarity from category norms for British participants (Hampton & Gardiner, 1983) were used to predict mean categorization times for 531words in 12 semantic categories. PF and typicality (but not familiarity) were found to make significant and independent contributions to categorization time. Error rates were related only to typicality (apart from errors made to ambiguous or unknown items). Experiment 2 provided a further dissociation of PF and typicality. Manipulating the difficulty of the task through the relatedness of the false items interacted primarily with the effect of typicality on categorization time, whereas, under conditions of easy discrimination, prior exposure to the category exemplars affected only the contribution of PF to the decision time. The dissociation of typicality and PF measures is interpreted as providing evidence that speeded categorization involves both retrieval of associations indexed by PF and a similarity-based decision process indexed by typicality.The phenomenon of gradedness within categories is the finding that some instances of common taxonomic categories (e.g., robin as a bird) are consistently judged as more typical or representative of their categories than are others (Barsalou, 1985;Hampton, 1979;Rosch, 1975). Typical instances have been shown to receive preferential processing in a wide range of cognitive tasks (for a review, see Hampton, 1993). For example, in a speeded categorization task, typical words are categorized more rapidly and more accurately than are atypical words (Hampton, 1979;Smith, Shoben, & Rips, 1974). Typical instances also tend to be generated with a high production frequency when people are asked to retrieve examples of categories from memory (Battig & Montague, 1969;Hampton & Gardiner, 1983;Mervis, Catlin, & Rosch, 1976).There are two fundamentally different ways of interpreting gradedness effects in common taxonomic categories. One is in terms of the learning history of the individual, and it proposes that "good" category members are those that have been most often associated with the category in the past. For example, nonanalytic models of category learning and concept representation (e.g., Brooks, 1978Brooks, , 1987 would emphasize the importance of past associations in determining speed of categorization. The other interpretation is in terms of what Rosch (1975) The author thanks Larry Barsalou, John Gardiner, Margaret Gardiner, David Green, Frank Keil, Robert Lorch, Michael McCloskey, and anonymous reviewers for comments and suggestions on the research, and Andy Ojukwu and Bill Fitzpatrick for assistance in data collection. Correspondence should be addressed to the author at the Psychology De...