1964
DOI: 10.1038/201544a0
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Determination of Distance by Echolocating Bats

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
22
0

Year Published

1965
1965
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 57 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 5 publications
0
22
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, at some point before flying through the hole, or crashing into the net, the bats abruptly increased sonar signal duration, which M. E. Jensen, C. F. Moss and A. Surlykke Acoustic landmarks and orientation by bats resulted in an overlap between the outgoing cry and the net echo. Since bats that use frequency modulated sonar vocalizations actively avoid pulse-echo overlap under most conditions (Cahlander et al, 1964;Kalko and Schnitzler, 1989;Hartley, 1992), the abrupt increase in cry duration indicates that the bat shifted its acoustic gaze to a position further away, i.e. to the other side of the net.…”
Section: Acoustic Behaviormentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…However, at some point before flying through the hole, or crashing into the net, the bats abruptly increased sonar signal duration, which M. E. Jensen, C. F. Moss and A. Surlykke Acoustic landmarks and orientation by bats resulted in an overlap between the outgoing cry and the net echo. Since bats that use frequency modulated sonar vocalizations actively avoid pulse-echo overlap under most conditions (Cahlander et al, 1964;Kalko and Schnitzler, 1989;Hartley, 1992), the abrupt increase in cry duration indicates that the bat shifted its acoustic gaze to a position further away, i.e. to the other side of the net.…”
Section: Acoustic Behaviormentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Spectrograms were made of 256 points Fast Fourier Transform (FFT), using a Hanning window and 45% overlap between consecutive FFTs. Bats using frequency modulated (FM) signals reduce signal duration as they approach an object, thus continually avoiding an overlap between outgoing cry and returning echo (Cahlander et al, 1964;Kalko and Schnitzler, 1989;Hartley, 1992). The time, or space, in which such an overlap would occur, has been referred to as the pulse-echo overlap zone (Kalko and Schnitzler, 1993) or the 'inner window' (Wilson and Moss, 2004).…”
Section: Sound Analysismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Bats reduce call duration to avoid pulse-echo overlap with nearby objects (Cahlander et al, 1964;Kalko and Schnitzler, 1989;Schnitzler et al, 1987), widen call bandwidth to better localize objects (Faure and Barclay, 1994;Hartley, 1992;Kalko and Schnitzler, 1993;Surlykke et al, 1993), reduce call intensity as they approach objects as a method for keeping target echo strength constant (Hartley, 1992;Hiryu et al, 2007), and decrease directionality to widen the field of view as they attack prey (Jakobsen and Surlykke, 2010). These adaptations occur as the bat adjusts the timing of sonar sounds, and may also be influenced by the temporal dynamics of respiration and flight kinematics.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Detecting prey, parsing the acoustic scene and localizing objects require sonar signals tailored to each task. Bats that emit short-frequency-modulated calls of low duty cycle to capture insects on the wing reduce the duration of calls in cluttered environments to minimize pulse-echo overlap (Cahlander et al, 1964;Kalko and Schnitzler, 1989;Schnitzler et al, 1987) and widen call bandwidth to increase information about object location (Faure and Barclay, 1994;Hartley, 1992;Jensen and Miller, 1999;Kalko and Schnitzler, 1993;Surlykke et al, 1993). These bats also adjust the interval between successive calls to receive echoes from relevant objects before producing the next call (Moss and Surlykke, 2001;Surlykke and Moss, 2000).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%