2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.08.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The development of the illusion of control and sense of agency in 7- to-12-year old children and adults

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
25
1

Year Published

2016
2016
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 37 publications
(26 citation statements)
references
References 69 publications
0
25
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This likely raised the level of the emotions until the moment of the discovery of the bad outcome: the higher the emotions before knowing it, the higher the difference between "before" and "after" the decision. However, we did not find an age effect, as found by [12], and this could be probably attributed to the different research paradigms used in the two studies.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 81%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This likely raised the level of the emotions until the moment of the discovery of the bad outcome: the higher the emotions before knowing it, the higher the difference between "before" and "after" the decision. However, we did not find an age effect, as found by [12], and this could be probably attributed to the different research paradigms used in the two studies.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 81%
“…This illusion is so pervasive that it lays at the heart of superstitious thinking also in the adult world. As regards the development of the sense of agency, researches have shown that young children present a reduced awareness about the possibilities to determine outcomes through actions, and that this altered sense of agency drives them easily towards the illusion of control [11,12]. The valence of the outcome plays a role as well, as young children tend to consider an outcome as intended when it satisfies their desire [13], and it has been also shown that the valence of the outcome drives children's judgements and decisions in bargaining games -see for example [14 -16].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We also manipulated control by asking participants to work on an unsolvable anagram task (Study 5) and by presenting participants with short vignettes describing situations associated with low feelings of control (Study 6). We investigated the effects of our control threat manipulations on magical thinking (Study 1-3), which was operationalized as the perception of an illusory correlation between two objectively unrelated events (van Elk, Rutjens, et al, 2015). We also investigated the effects on general belief in conspiracy theories and in paranormal beliefs as a source of compensatory control (Study 4 and 5).…”
Section: Overview Of Experiments and Approachmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Because such experiences can be psychologically stressful, it has been suggested that people have a motivated tendency to counter such feelings through different psychological mechanisms (Landau, Kay, & Whitson, 2015). For instance, people may tend to overestimate the extent to which they can control their lives or their environment (Presson & Benassi, 1996;Whalen, 1998), as becomes evident in research on positive illusions and magical thinking (Taylor & Brown, 1988;van Elk, Rutjens, & van der Pligt, 2015). Furthermore, people who lack control may try to restore their implicit sense of control through attributional biases (Pittman & Pittman, 1979).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation