1959
DOI: 10.1037/h0045525
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The distance gradient in kinesthetic figural aftereffect.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

3
21
0

Year Published

1966
1966
1979
1979

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(24 citation statements)
references
References 5 publications
3
21
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Under the conditioning model, a strong unconditioned stimulus should be expected to produce a stronger conditioned response, but the H90-VI 0 inspection block produced a numerically-though not significantly-weaker aftereffect than the H70-V30 block. On the other hand, as noted earlier, the relationship between the magnitude of the contingent aftereffect and the dimensions of the inspection blocks is consistent with the results of studies of simple kinesthetic figural aftereffects (Charles & Duncan, 1959;Kohler & Dinnerstein, 1947). Thus, the present aftereffect may display more of the characteristics of sensory processes and fewer characteristics of conditioning.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 78%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Under the conditioning model, a strong unconditioned stimulus should be expected to produce a stronger conditioned response, but the H90-VI 0 inspection block produced a numerically-though not significantly-weaker aftereffect than the H70-V30 block. On the other hand, as noted earlier, the relationship between the magnitude of the contingent aftereffect and the dimensions of the inspection blocks is consistent with the results of studies of simple kinesthetic figural aftereffects (Charles & Duncan, 1959;Kohler & Dinnerstein, 1947). Thus, the present aftereffect may display more of the characteristics of sensory processes and fewer characteristics of conditioning.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 78%
“…The repulsion reaches a maximum at some particular separation between test and inspection contours, declining at higher and lower levels of separation. But even when the test and inspection contours coincide, the inspection contours typically exert some influence, usually involving a reduction in the perceived size of a test object (for example, Charles & Duncan, 1959;Kohler & Dinnerstein, 1947;Kohler & Wallach, 1944).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Charles and Duncan (1959) have demonstrated a distance gradient in KAE; I stimuli either much larger or smaller than test (T) stimuli result in little or no KAE. The distance gradient is predictable on the basis of satiation theory (Kohler & Dinnerstein, 1947).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Two experiments investigated the effect of using a different finger for inspection (I) than is used in making judgments on the size of a kinesthetic aftereffect (KAE) Although the procedures for inducing kinesthetic after-effects (KAE) have been investigated for such variables as size of the inspection (1) stimulus (Charles & Duncan, 1959), amount and distribution of I trials (Carlson, 1963), and attention to the I stimulus (Bakan & Thompson, 1963), there has been no investigation of the role of the fingers used during I stimulation and for judgments on the size of KAE. Charles and Duncan (1959) have demonstrated a distance gradient in KAE; I stimuli either much larger or smaller than test (T) stimuli result in little or no KAE.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%