2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.02.011
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The effects of bilingualism on conflict monitoring, cognitive control, and garden-path recovery

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

8
60
0
1

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 71 publications
(69 citation statements)
references
References 69 publications
8
60
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Based on previous studies examining such links at other linguistic levels (e.g., Blumenfeld & Marian, 2011; 2013; Mercier et al, 2014; Teubner-Rhodes et al, 2016), we predicted that more efficient performance on the Stroop task would be related to decreased competition from the ‘e’ phonotactic constraint for bilinguals. In the bilingual group, a significant positive correlation was observed between the Stroop effect and the CPCF effect ( r = .45, p = .039).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Based on previous studies examining such links at other linguistic levels (e.g., Blumenfeld & Marian, 2011; 2013; Mercier et al, 2014; Teubner-Rhodes et al, 2016), we predicted that more efficient performance on the Stroop task would be related to decreased competition from the ‘e’ phonotactic constraint for bilinguals. In the bilingual group, a significant positive correlation was observed between the Stroop effect and the CPCF effect ( r = .45, p = .039).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The involvement of inhibitory control skills during bilingual language processing has been documented across various tasks indexing phonological (Blumenfeld & Marian, 2011, 2013; Blumenfeld, Schroeder, Bobb, Freeman, & Marian, 2016; Mercier, Pivneva, & Titone, 2014), lexical (Linck, Hoshino, & Kroll, 2008; Linck, Schwieter, & Sunderman, 2012; Prior & Gollan, 2011), semantic (Martín, Macizo, & Bajo, 2010), and syntactic co-activation (Linck, Hoshino, & Kroll, 2008; Teubner-Rhodes et al, 2016; see Freeman, Shook, & Marian, 2016, for review). In the current study, we seek to examine the relation between cognitive control and cross-linguistic competition resolution (i.e., how individuals manage and suppress interfering cues while focusing on relevant information) at the sublexical level.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Accordingly, recent studies suggest that sentence processing, and in particular garden-path recovery, can be improved by training nonlinguistic conflict resolution (see Novick, Hussey, Teubner-Rhodes, Harbison & Bunting 2013). Crucially, Teubner-Rhodes, Mishler, Corbett, Andreu, Sanz-Torrent, Trueswell and Novick (2016) show that bilingual participants are more accurate than monolinguals in non-linguistic conflict resolution tasks and in sentence comprehension, both before and after training. These studies suggest that the relationship between general cognition and language is a complex one and that the processes involved in the two do share some common characteristics.…”
Section: Bilingual Language Processing and Cognitionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Morales and colleagues (Morales, Gomez-Ariza, & Bajo, 2013; Morales, Yudes, Gomez-Ariza, & Bajo, 2015) reported better performance by bilinguals than monolinguals on this task and argued that the bilinguals had greater capacity to monitor these components of executive function. Finally, as part of a larger study of conflict monitoring in sentence processing, Teubner-Rhodes et al (2016) demonstrated better performance by bilinguals than monolinguals on a nonverbal N-back task that they describe as requiring conflict monitoring.…”
Section: Evidence For Bilingual Effects On Cognitionmentioning
confidence: 99%