1972
DOI: 10.1177/002221947200500501
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Effects of Kindergarten Instruction in Alphabet and Numbers on First Grade Reading

Abstract: The ability to recognize letters and numbers in kindergarten has been demonstrated to be a better predictor of end-of-first-grade reading skills than other accepted "readiness skills" or IQ. It would thus seem logical to hypothesize that instruction in recognizing letters and numbers in kindergarten should produce an increment to reading level attained at the end of first grade. To test this possibility, four classes of kindergarten children were given the Letters and Numbers Subtest of the Gates Reading Readi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

2
7
0

Year Published

1973
1973
1995
1995

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 8 publications
2
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The present finding adds to the already impressive research evidence relating the role of this skill in predicting short-term reading performance at first-grade level (Wilson & Flemming, 1938a, Wilson & Flemming, 1940Wilson, 1942;Gavel, 1958;Weiner & Feldman, 1963;Di Nello, 1965;Barrett, 1965;Silvaroli, 1965;De Hirsch et al, 1966;Muehl & Kremenack, 1966;Bond & Dykstra, 1967;Lowell, 1971;Hick & Santman, 1971;Askov, et al, 1972;Silberberg et al, 1972). Without exception naming letters proved to be the best single predictor, either in simple or multiple prediction.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 66%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The present finding adds to the already impressive research evidence relating the role of this skill in predicting short-term reading performance at first-grade level (Wilson & Flemming, 1938a, Wilson & Flemming, 1940Wilson, 1942;Gavel, 1958;Weiner & Feldman, 1963;Di Nello, 1965;Barrett, 1965;Silvaroli, 1965;De Hirsch et al, 1966;Muehl & Kremenack, 1966;Bond & Dykstra, 1967;Lowell, 1971;Hick & Santman, 1971;Askov, et al, 1972;Silberberg et al, 1972). Without exception naming letters proved to be the best single predictor, either in simple or multiple prediction.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 66%
“…Despite instruction, high IQ and upper middle-class background, the Ss still showed marked differences in letter naming at the end of the instructional program. Silberberg (1972) reported similar differences in 5s' ability to learn letters and numbers after eight weeks of instruction and practice. Di Nello (1965) found almost half of his male first graders named less than half the letters on a September readiness test.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 74%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…While the educational benefits associated with the LEA appear to be many (Hacker, 1980;Hall, 1981;Harste, Woodward & Burke, 1984;Jensen & Hanson, 1982;Lee & Allen, 1963;Sulzby, 1980Sulzby, , 1985, there are some kindergarten teachers, parents, and reading researchers (Barrett, 1965;Becker & Gersten, 1982;Haddock, 1976;Johnson & Baumann, 1984;Muehl, 1962;Pflaum, Walberg, Karegianes & Rasher, 1980;Samuels, 1972;Silberberg, Silberberg & Iverson, 1972;Williams, 1985) who are not convinced that language experience instruction will develop the literacy subskills required for success in beginning reading programs; their concern is that traditional LEA procedures do not include systematic instruction in subskill areas, such as sound/symbol relationships, visual discrimination, auditory discrimination, and use of context clues. As a reaction to this criticism, proponents of language experience instruction have offered suggestions for expanding and varying the use of the traditional LEA so that it includes subskill instruction, but few studies have examined empirically the effects of specific LEA expansions, or variations, on emergent literacy skills.…”
Section: Language and Educationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In support of language/meaning instruction, research by several educators indicates the important roles played by prior knowledge (i.e., experience) and information processing during the process of obtaining meaning from text (Chomsky, 1972;Dunn, 198 I;Durkin, 1966Durkin, , 1974Harste, Woodward, & Burke, 1984;Heath, 1982Heath, , 1983Hirsch, 1985;Mason, 1984;Menyuk, 1988;Michaels, 198 I;Olson, 1984;Pearson, Hanson, & Gordon, 1979;Soderberg, 1977;Teale & Sulzby, 1986;Wells, 1981;Wilson & Anderson, 1985). There is also a great deal of research evidence which SUppOt1S subskill instruction, particularly in the areas of visual discrimination skills and phonics skills (Barrett, 1965;Becker & Gersten, 1982;Bond & Dykstra, 1967;Chal\, 1983;Haddock, 1976;Jeffrey & Samuels, 1967;Johnson & Baumann, 1984;Muehl, 1962;Olson, 1958;Pflaum, Walberg, Karegianes, & Rasher, 1980;Rosenshine & Stevens, 1984;Samuels, 1972;Silberberg, Silberberg, & Iverson, 1972;Venezky, 1975;Williams, 1985). Given the inconclusive nature of research findings on the issue of language/meaning emphasis versus subskill emphasis to aid the development of emergent literacy skills, it may be that the best strategy would be to combine elements of language/meaning and subskills instruction into a single, unified approach.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%