2017
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00888
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Effects of Spatial Endogenous Pre-cueing across Eccentricities

Abstract: Frequently, we use expectations about likely locations of a target to guide the allocation of our attention. Despite the importance of this attentional process in everyday tasks, examination of pre-cueing effects on attention, particularly endogenous pre-cueing effects, has been relatively little explored outside an eccentricity of 20°. Given the visual field has functional subdivisions that attentional processes can differ significantly among the foveal, perifoveal, and more peripheral areas, how endogenous p… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

1
3
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 86 publications
(121 reference statements)
1
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Possibly eccentricity acts in a similar manner, in that it operates on the values in the priority map through spatial gain control, such that less eccentric items receive stronger weights than those presented further away. This idea bears much resemblance to the central bias notion as proposed by Wolfe et al ( 1998 ) to account for the eccentricity effects in their study (see also Feng & Spence, 2017 ) and is also consistent with our finding that both target and distractor eccentricity affect capture. However, our results also showed that the effects of target eccentricity and distractor eccentricity were largely additive – in other words, the eccentricity of one item did not modulate the effect of the eccentricity of the other item.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…Possibly eccentricity acts in a similar manner, in that it operates on the values in the priority map through spatial gain control, such that less eccentric items receive stronger weights than those presented further away. This idea bears much resemblance to the central bias notion as proposed by Wolfe et al ( 1998 ) to account for the eccentricity effects in their study (see also Feng & Spence, 2017 ) and is also consistent with our finding that both target and distractor eccentricity affect capture. However, our results also showed that the effects of target eccentricity and distractor eccentricity were largely additive – in other words, the eccentricity of one item did not modulate the effect of the eccentricity of the other item.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…Either, cueing effects rise with eccentricity (cueing near periphery < cueing medium periphery < cueing far periphery. This would constitute a quantitative difference between near, medium, and far periphery and is supported by rising cueing effects found in the near and medium periphery (e.g., [15,34,36] and several studies using peripheral cues in combination with a detection task failed to reveal cueing effects beyond the EOMR [16,[45][46][47]. This would constitute a qualitative difference between near and medium and the far periphery.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 83%
“…Nonetheless, knowledge about visual attention is restricted by an often-neglected factor: The size of commercially available computer screens. The majority of attention studies used target eccentricities up to 25 • of visual angle [10][11][12], in the rarer cases up to 30 • [13][14][15]. To my knowledge, only two studies, Casteau and Smith [16] and Poggel, Strasburger and MacKeben [17] investigated visual attention beyond 30 • [16], used eccentricities up to 44 • , and [17] even up to 60 • .…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation