2015
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00483
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The embodied dynamics of perceptual causality: a slippery slope?

Abstract: In Michotte's launching displays, while the launcher (object A) seems to move autonomously, the target (object B) seems to be displaced passively. However, the impression of A actively launching B does not persist beyond a certain distance identified as the “radius of action” of A over B. If the target keeps moving beyond the radius of action, it loses its passivity and seems to move autonomously. Here, we manipulated implied friction by drawing (or not) a surface upon which A and B are traveling, and by varyi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 60 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…3B) on a table with the image facing upward (toward the ceiling) and viewed from above by the participants. Such a manipulation would induce an expectation of gravity when the stimulus moved from the upper to the lower edge of the display in the vertical screen orientation, but not when it moved in the horizontal screen orientation (Amorim et al, 2015).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…3B) on a table with the image facing upward (toward the ceiling) and viewed from above by the participants. Such a manipulation would induce an expectation of gravity when the stimulus moved from the upper to the lower edge of the display in the vertical screen orientation, but not when it moved in the horizontal screen orientation (Amorim et al, 2015).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A separate representation of air drag has the additional advantage that it can be employed for both vertical and horizontal movement components, while an integrated gravity and air drag prior is only viable for free fall. While evidence for the existence of a friction prior for movement on surfaces has been presented (Hubbard, 1995 ; Amorim et al, 2015 ), the role of air resistance and drag-relevant features of objects in perception remain to be studied more systematically.…”
Section: Attunement To Earth Gravity: Interception Performance Under mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, it is important to perceive how the force of friction acts upon objects, both to predict when they will slow down (Amorim et al, 2015;Hubbard, 1998), but also to predict the onset of movement. Again, imagine a stationary car spinning its wheels in the mudhere it is critical to form a sensitive model of the wheel's frictive interaction with the ground, in order to predict where the car will move next.…”
Section: The Current Study: An Implicit Model Of Friction Drives Spat...mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, if the second object continues to move over a great distance (outside the launch's 'radius of action'), then this movement will instead appear driven by an internal force (see also Boyle, 1961;Yela, 1954). For objects seen moving along higher friction surfaces, there is a reduced 'radius of action' beyond which the object begins to look self-propelled (Amorim et al, 2015). Thus, visual impressions of self-propelledness outside of the radius of action suggest the implicit logic that, as an object moves, the force of friction tends to cause it to slow down.…”
Section: Seeing Friction As a Cause Of Movementmentioning
confidence: 99%