2017
DOI: 10.1177/1948550617699251
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Face of Suspicion

Abstract: Because Whites use positivity to conceal bias, people of color may question whether Whites’ positivity is genuine. We predicted that those suspicious of Whites’ motives may mentally represent Whites as less trustworthy and more hostile than those low in suspicion. We tested these predictions using reverse correlation. First, we examined high- and low-suspicion Black participants’ mental representations of Whites using neutrally expressed (Study 1a) and smiling (Study 2a) White base faces. In Study 2b, we compa… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
11
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

3
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
0
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It is important that brief exposures to crowds (1/3 second each) influenced racial cognition in a predictable manner, but this occurred after exposure to a number of trials (i.e., 216 trials) and the statistically significant effects on racial categorization were on the order of only 5% of total biracial categorizations (i.e., 1 of 21 faces). This effect, however, is still meaningful and not unusual for this area of research (Chen & Hamilton, 2012; Hoffman, Trawalter, Axt, & Oliver, 2016; Kubota, Peiso, Marcum, & Cloutier, 2017; Lloyd, Kunstman, Tuscherer, & Bernstein, 2017). Applying the mechanism we isolated in Study 1 to a lifetime of exposure to crowds could, in theory, have much larger influences on racial category boundaries.…”
Section: Study 2: Do People Encounter Emotional Segregation In Natura...mentioning
confidence: 82%
“…It is important that brief exposures to crowds (1/3 second each) influenced racial cognition in a predictable manner, but this occurred after exposure to a number of trials (i.e., 216 trials) and the statistically significant effects on racial categorization were on the order of only 5% of total biracial categorizations (i.e., 1 of 21 faces). This effect, however, is still meaningful and not unusual for this area of research (Chen & Hamilton, 2012; Hoffman, Trawalter, Axt, & Oliver, 2016; Kubota, Peiso, Marcum, & Cloutier, 2017; Lloyd, Kunstman, Tuscherer, & Bernstein, 2017). Applying the mechanism we isolated in Study 1 to a lifetime of exposure to crowds could, in theory, have much larger influences on racial category boundaries.…”
Section: Study 2: Do People Encounter Emotional Segregation In Natura...mentioning
confidence: 82%
“…Using happiness ratings, however, makes it difficult to ascertain the extent to which participants were able to differentiate between true and false smiles. Therefore, in the present study, we used a signal detection paradigm (Kunstman et al, 2016; Lloyd, Kunstman, Tuscherer, & Bernstein, 2017) to specifically investigate racial biases in categorizing expressions as true or false smiles. Experiment 4 also included a sample of non-Black participants.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, there are robust own-race advantages in face recognition (Hugenberg et al, 2010), anxiety detection (Gray et al, 2008), emotion recognition (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002), and smile authenticity (Friesen et al, 2019). Similarly, advantages for high status and majority group members are documented in face memory (Ratcliff et al, 2011), emotion recognition (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002), and lie detection (Lloyd et al, 2017). Because Black Americans are low-status minority group members in the United States (Fiske, 2012) and are commonly treated by White providers (∼75% of U.S. doctors are White; Castillo-Page, 2010), we theorized Black individuals are at greater risk for having their pain expressions misunderstood (Fiske, 2012).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%