2000
DOI: 10.1016/s0301-0511(00)00062-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The gap effect in pro-saccades and anti-saccades in psychometric schizotypes

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
13
0

Year Published

2004
2004
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
2
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In the current study of healthy, young adults the IQ range was restricted and the SPQ scores were both restricted and low (see also Klein et al, 2000). Restriction of range may have limited our ability to resolve the relationships between the clinical and saccadic variables.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 65%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In the current study of healthy, young adults the IQ range was restricted and the SPQ scores were both restricted and low (see also Klein et al, 2000). Restriction of range may have limited our ability to resolve the relationships between the clinical and saccadic variables.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 65%
“…This coupling suggests that (1) participants tend to use one of two strategies for performing antisaccade tasks: to emphasize speed or to maximize accuracy; and/or (2) that error rate may be secondary to speed of saccade generation (Klein et al, 2000). This pattern in healthy (Ethridge et al, 2009; Ettinger et al, 2005, Evdokimidis et al, 2002) and in schizophrenia (Harris et al, 2006) participants suggests that heightened visual orienting to a salient peripheral stimulus, manifested in speeded saccade latencies, may predispose to greater risk of antisaccade error generation independent of psychiatric status.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, the no-overlap version of the task may have some advantages: in contrast to the overlap version of the task, with the no-overlap when the antisaccade cue is presented, no stimulus is present and visual attention is not actively engaged. Suppression of a prepotent response, which is required for correct performance on the antisaccade task, may be more difficult during a task situation where attention is not engaged (Klein, Brügner, Foerster, Müller, & Schweickhardt, 2000) than in a task situation where attention is engaged (i.e., during the overlap version). In support of this suggestion, Curtis, Calkins, and Iacono (2001) administered two versions of the antisaccade task (an overlap and a no-overlap version) to schizophrenia patients, their biological relatives, and controls, and they found that the no-overlap task produced greater differences between relatives and controls.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Concerning the observation of antisaccade deficits in populations at increased genetic risk for schizophrenia, numerous studies link elevated levels of schizotypal traits to increased antisaccade error rates on step antisaccade tasks in non-clinical samples (Ettinger et al, 2005b;Gooding, 1999;Holahan & O'Driscoll, 2005;Larrison, Ferrante, Briand, & Sereno, 2000;O'Driscoll, Lenzenweger, & Holzman, 1998;Smyrnis et al, 2003; but see O'Driscoll, 2005, andKlein, Brügner, Foerster, Müller, &Schweickhardt, 2000, for a failure to replicate this relationship using gap and overlap tasks). This deficit may be related more closely to positive than negative schizotypal symptoms (Ettinger et al, 2005b;Holahan & O'Driscoll, 2005) and is not due to increased levels of general psychopathology or neuroticism (Ettinger et al, 2005b;Smyrnis et al, 2003).…”
Section: Do Antisaccade Deficits Mark Genetic Liability For Schizophrmentioning
confidence: 99%