2015
DOI: 10.1068/p7940
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Headscarf Effect Revisited: Further Evidence for a Culture-Based Internal Face Processing Advantage

Abstract: Encoding the internal features of unfamiliar faces poses a perceptual challenge that occasionally results in face recognition errors. Extensive experience with faces framed by a headscarf may, however, enhance perceivers' ability to process internal facial information. To examine this claim empirically, participants in the United Arab Emirates and the United States of America completed a standard part-whole face recognition task. Accuracy on the task was examined using a 2 (perceiver culture: Emirati vs Americ… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
11
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 31 publications
(41 reference statements)
0
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Parallel lines of work in the social domain examining how emotion recognition may be affected by cultural face coverings, such as a niqab (Fischer et al 2012;Kret and de Gelder, 2012;Kret & Fischer, 2018;Wang et al 2015;Hareli et al, 2013), report similar results. There was a reduction in the accuracy of emotion perception for participants wearing a niqab for happiness but no impact for recognizing anger, which depends on perceiving upper parts of the face (i.e., the eyes).…”
Section: Emotion Recognition and Facial Occlusionmentioning
confidence: 76%
“…Parallel lines of work in the social domain examining how emotion recognition may be affected by cultural face coverings, such as a niqab (Fischer et al 2012;Kret and de Gelder, 2012;Kret & Fischer, 2018;Wang et al 2015;Hareli et al, 2013), report similar results. There was a reduction in the accuracy of emotion perception for participants wearing a niqab for happiness but no impact for recognizing anger, which depends on perceiving upper parts of the face (i.e., the eyes).…”
Section: Emotion Recognition and Facial Occlusionmentioning
confidence: 76%
“…We also assessed participants' preoccupation with COVID-19 and their exposure to face masks. To this end, we developed four face-valid items to measure preoccupation with COVID-19 and adapted items by Wang and colleagues [52] to measure exposure to face masks. As these scales were not relevant for the analyses, we do not discuss them further.…”
Section: Materials and Proceduresmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We obtained permission to retrieve and adapt these photographs from the FACES database [51]. As all photographs in the databank depict native German community members, FACES stimuli were particularly suited for the purpose of the present investigation [52]. The stimuli used in the critical trials systematically differed in age (young, middle-aged, old), gender (female, male), and expressed emotion (neutral, happy, fearful, angry, sad, disgusted).…”
Section: Materials and Proceduresmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous studies found that the recognition and matching of unfamiliar faces rely heavily on external features (Ellis et al, 1979;Young et al, 1985;O'Donnell and Bruce, 2001), whereas familiar faces can be easily recognized and discriminated one from another based on internal features (Ellis et al, 1979;Henderson et al, 2005;Sporer and Horry, 2011; but see Toseeb et al, 2014;Toseeb et al, 2012). Some authors have argued that perceptual expertise is required to successfully encode internal face features (Megreya and Bindemann, 2009;Megreya et al, 2012;Wang et al, 2015), with developmental studies finding that adult-like processing of internal features is not achieved until between 10 and 15 years of age (Campbell et al, 1999;Want et al, 2003; but see Bonner et al, 2004).…”
Section: The External Features and Orbmentioning
confidence: 99%