1992
DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1992.tb00978.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The relationship between causal attributions and judgements of the typicality of events described by sentences

Abstract: Three experiments examined the influence of causal attributions on people's judgements of what is a typical event, as reflected in their judgements of what is a typical sentence. In Expt 1, college students judged the typicality of an event depicted by a sentence. In Expt 2, students judged which of two participants caused the action or state depicted in a sentence. In both experiments, the animacy of the participants interacted with different classes of action or experiencer verbs in similar ways to influence… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
20
0

Year Published

1994
1994
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(24 citation statements)
references
References 10 publications
4
20
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In addition, properties of the participants affect implicit causality. Changing the gender (Lafrance, Brownell, & Hahn, 1997), animacy (Corrigan, 1988(Corrigan, , 1992, or typicality (Corrigan, 1992;Garvey et al, 1976) of the participants changes the implicit-causality bias, as do contextual factors that affect focus (Majid, Sanford, & Pickering, 2006). Finally, syntactic form is important, with causal attribution differing for active versus passive constructions (Au, 1986;Garvey et al, 1976;Kasof & Lee, 1994).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition, properties of the participants affect implicit causality. Changing the gender (Lafrance, Brownell, & Hahn, 1997), animacy (Corrigan, 1988(Corrigan, , 1992, or typicality (Corrigan, 1992;Garvey et al, 1976) of the participants changes the implicit-causality bias, as do contextual factors that affect focus (Majid, Sanford, & Pickering, 2006). Finally, syntactic form is important, with causal attribution differing for active versus passive constructions (Au, 1986;Garvey et al, 1976;Kasof & Lee, 1994).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This debate prompted a series of studies showing that aside from verb-level argument structure (Brown and Fish 1983;Hartshorne and Snedeker 2013), various factors (both linguistic and extra-linguistic) can modulate the implicit causality bias, including the negativity of its connotative meaning (Semin and Marsman 1994), the gender and typicality of participants (LaFrance et al 1997;Corrigan 1992), quantifier-induced focus (Majid et al 2006), as well as the availability of explanations in context (Bott and Solstad 2013). Taken together, these studies support the view that the implicit causality bias is due to information coming from multiple dimensions, both at the level of the verb, as well as in discourse.…”
Section: Implicit Causality and Re-mention Biasesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast, a fragment such as John praised Mary becauseƒ typically leads participants to continue with a reference to Mary, because they likely assume that Mary has done something praiseworthy. A considerable literature has catalogued this implicit causality bias (e.g., Au, 1986;Brown & Fish, 1983;Corrigan, 1988Corrigan, , 1992Garvey & Caramazza, 1974). Such studies assume that interpersonal verbs invite spontaneous causal attribution.…”
Section: Do People Go Beyond the Information Given?mentioning
confidence: 99%