2008
DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00748.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The relationship between second‐order false belief and display rules reasoning: the integration of cognitive and affective social understanding

Abstract: To investigate the relation between cognitive and affective social understanding, Japanese 4- to 8-year-olds received tasks of first- and second-order false beliefs and prosocial and self-presentational display rules. From 6 to 8 years, children comprehended display rules, as well as second-order false belief, using social pressures justifications decreasingly and motivational justifications with embedded perspectives increasingly with age. Although not related to either type of display across ages, second-ord… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

12
50
1

Year Published

2010
2010
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 39 publications
(63 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
12
50
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Growing up in a crowded urban camp with no privacy, pemulung children are socialized to be open and unpretentious and therefore may have little experience of hidden emotion. This interpretation fits with the finding that in Japan, where privacy and self-control are highly valued, children pass hidden-emotion tasks earlier than their Western counterparts (Naito & Seki, 2009).…”
Section: Cross-cultural Variations In the Sequential Acquisition Of Msupporting
confidence: 73%
“…Growing up in a crowded urban camp with no privacy, pemulung children are socialized to be open and unpretentious and therefore may have little experience of hidden emotion. This interpretation fits with the finding that in Japan, where privacy and self-control are highly valued, children pass hidden-emotion tasks earlier than their Western counterparts (Naito & Seki, 2009).…”
Section: Cross-cultural Variations In the Sequential Acquisition Of Msupporting
confidence: 73%
“…Japanese children’s performance on the Japanese false-belief task was comparable to their performance on the English false-belief task. These results are consistent with the most recent study that tested Japanese children with the second-order false-belief task [65]. In this study, despite the significant developmental delay in the first-order false-belief task (see also [66, 67]), Japanese children’s onset of the second-order false-belief task was about the same as the A / E children’s (i.e., between 6 and 9 years old [68, 69]).…”
Section: Evidence From Behavioral Studiessupporting
confidence: 93%
“…In addition, the scores for both contexts in the verbal display task were significantly or marginally higher when children gave correct answers for the second-order false belief task than when they gave incorrect answers (Table 2). These results do not conflict with previous findings (e.g., Broomfield et al, 2002;Naito & Seki, The scores were reversed to create high scores when participants selected utterances with positive or neutral emotion.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 75%
“…The child is asked to infer both the protagonist's real emotion and his or her apparent facial expression. The age at which children can distinguish between real and apparent emotion varies a bit, depending upon whether the children must spontaneously appreciate that the protagonist wishes to conceal his or her emotion (e.g., Saarni, 1979) or they are explicitly told the protagonist's motive (e.g., Harris et al, 1986); however, this research has shown that it is difficult for children to understand this distinction until the age of 6 to 8 years (Naito & Seki, 2009). …”
mentioning
confidence: 97%