2008
DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01150.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Role of Intentionality and Iconicity in Children’s Developing Comprehension and Production of Cartographic Symbols

Abstract: The contribution of intentionality understanding to symbolic development was examined. Actors added colored dots to a map, displaying either symbolic or aesthetic intentions. In Study 1, most children (5-6 years) understood actors' intentions, but when asked which graphic would help find hidden objects, most selected the incorrect (aesthetic) one whose dot color matched referent color. On a similar task in Study 2, 5- and 6-year-olds systematically picked incorrectly, 9- and 10-year-olds picked correctly, and … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
34
0
1

Year Published

2009
2009
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
3
3
2

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 34 publications
(38 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
3
34
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, we did not administer some preliminary labeling tasks (see Browne & Woolley, 2001) and the child age range in our study was wider (3;3 to 6;2 rather than 3;11 to 5;3). Nonetheless, our intention-resemblance findings are supported by numerous studies indicating that even older children have difficulty reasoning about intentionality when it is not aligned with resemblance in tasks that involve producing and/or using symbols such as drawings, simple maps and 3D models (Liben & Downs, 1989;Myers & Liben, 2008; see also Bloom, 1996). The results of this study support the idea that, although preschool and kindergarten children have an understanding of mind, this ability continues to develop during the school years (Carpendale & Chandler, 1996;Lalonde & Chandler, 2002).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 86%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, we did not administer some preliminary labeling tasks (see Browne & Woolley, 2001) and the child age range in our study was wider (3;3 to 6;2 rather than 3;11 to 5;3). Nonetheless, our intention-resemblance findings are supported by numerous studies indicating that even older children have difficulty reasoning about intentionality when it is not aligned with resemblance in tasks that involve producing and/or using symbols such as drawings, simple maps and 3D models (Liben & Downs, 1989;Myers & Liben, 2008; see also Bloom, 1996). The results of this study support the idea that, although preschool and kindergarten children have an understanding of mind, this ability continues to develop during the school years (Carpendale & Chandler, 1996;Lalonde & Chandler, 2002).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 86%
“…However, under certain conditions, even older preschoolers may have difficulty reasoning about mental states in the context of using symbols. For example, when the creator's intentions are pitted against the symbol's resemblance (e.g., a green dot represents a red fire truck), even 5-and 6-year-olds have trouble using information about the creator's mental states to choose the right graphic representation that could help a friend find a toy (Myers & Liben, 2008). Taken together, studies on symbolic development show that children's understanding of their own and other people's intentions, beliefs, and knowledge plays a role in the development of their ability to communicate information through symbols.…”
Section: Understanding Of Mind and Symbolic Developmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…That is, the specific process of iToM is not a relevant one for communicating with abstract symbols. Instead, other aspects within the multifaceted construct of ToM (e.g., intention understanding; Myers & Liben, 2008) are relevant to abstract-symbol understanding. Additionally, in the current study, matrix reasoning emerged as a significant predictor of children's performance in the abstract (but not iconic) condition.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…In addition to scoring children's maps to provide a quantitative score (as just described), we also scored children's map keys qualitatively, following and extending earlier coding systems for children's map keys (Myers & Liben, 2008). Specifically, keys were coded as being categorical (a single symbol paired with a single notation explaining its meaning), redundant (the same symbol-referent link was specified each time the symbol appeared on the map), modified (the symbols were changed by drawing or writing on them; e.g., turning an abstract symbol into a miniature picture of its referent), or global (the key broadly explained the meaning of the symbols but lacked specific information; e.g., ''the stickers mean toys'').…”
Section: Mapping Tasksmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Most material structures lend themselves to several alternative meaning exploring interpretations (see, for example, Myers and Liben 2008), which consider attention and attunement to addresser's original intention crucial (see also Levinson 1979). for extensive references.…”
Section: Notesmentioning
confidence: 99%