2011
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0028999
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Role of Motor Learning in Spatial Adaptation near a Tool

Abstract: Some visual-tactile (bimodal) cells have visual receptive fields (vRFs) that overlap and extend moderately beyond the skin of the hand. Neurophysiological evidence suggests, however, that a vRF will grow to encompass a hand-held tool following active tool use but not after passive holding. Why does active tool use, and not passive holding, lead to spatial adaptation near a tool? We asked whether spatial adaptation could be the result of motor or visual experience with the tool, and we distinguished between the… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
22
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
(94 reference statements)
1
22
0
Order By: Relevance
“…were necessary for embodiment (Brown et al, 2011; Cardinali, Brozzoli, Luauté, et al, 2016; Rademaker et al, 2014), there should be no modulation of tactile perception on a stationary arm. In contrast to this prediction, visual feedback produced clear tool embodiment on the stationary left forearm.…”
Section: Experiments 1: Visual Illusion Of Tool Usementioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…were necessary for embodiment (Brown et al, 2011; Cardinali, Brozzoli, Luauté, et al, 2016; Rademaker et al, 2014), there should be no modulation of tactile perception on a stationary arm. In contrast to this prediction, visual feedback produced clear tool embodiment on the stationary left forearm.…”
Section: Experiments 1: Visual Illusion Of Tool Usementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Using a tool (as opposed to passive handling) has been found to be necessary for tool embodiment (Brown et al, 2011; Farnè et al, 2005; Garbarini et al, 2015; Maravita et al, 2002; Witt et al, 2005), underscoring the importance of use-related sensory and motor signals. In Experiment 4, we tested whether this was also the case for illusory tool use.…”
Section: Experiments 4: Passive Tool Holdingmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…However, a large amount of the literature supports the notion that the brain is capable to adapt when changes in the environment happen. For example, when an external force is applied to the arm of a subject asked to point to a target (Bhushan and Shadmehr 1999;Diedrichsen et al 2005;Ostry et al 2010) or when inertial properties of a tool are invisibly altered (Brown et al, 2011), the subject will show, at the beginning, a deviation from the desired and requested trajectory resulting in a discrepancy between the planned movement and the desired consequences. This discrepancy arises from the fact that the movement has been planned based on previous knowledge about the environment and/or the hand-held object (thus ignoring the unexpected perturbation).…”
Section: How Tools Challenge the Sensorimotor Systemmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This discrepancy arises from the fact that the movement has been planned based on previous knowledge about the environment and/or the hand-held object (thus ignoring the unexpected perturbation). By practicing the movement under the same (new) condition over and over, the subject will learn to compensate for the introduced force and eventually reach the desired level of performance (Bhushan and Shadmehr 1999;Diedrichsen et al 2005;Massen and Prinz 2009;Brown et al 2011). Recently, Ostry et al (2010) used a force Weld paradigm to study the potential consequences of motor learning on the perceptual domain.…”
Section: How Tools Challenge the Sensorimotor Systemmentioning
confidence: 99%