2013
DOI: 10.1007/s00426-013-0510-z
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The role of temporal delay and repeated prospective memory cue exposure on the deactivation of completed intentions

Abstract: Previous studies have shown that completed prospective memory (PM) intentions entail aftereffects in terms of ongoing-task-performance decrements in trials containing repeated PM cues which previously served as PM cues triggering the intended action. Previous research reported that PM aftereffects decrease over time, thus revealing a specific time course of PM aftereffects. In the present study, we tested two accounts for this pattern, assuming either that the decline of aftereffects is related to the temporal… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
17
0
2

Year Published

2013
2013
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 14 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
0
17
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Similar to previous studies, subjective consequences of TSST exposure died away relatively fast after cessation of the stressor, whereas neuroendocrine responses to stress were still present after 50 minutes [ 20 , 41 ]. Second, cognitive results also showed the predicted outcome: PM accuracy was within the normal range for PM studies (i.e., no ceiling effect occurred) and RTs and error rates were increased for PM REPEATED trials compared to standard trials, indicating aftereffects of completed intentions [ 3 , 4 ]. Most strikingly, however, despite a strong biological and subjective stress response and standard effects in the cognitive task, stress did not exert any influence on PM performance, ongoing-task performance, and aftereffects of completed intentions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Similar to previous studies, subjective consequences of TSST exposure died away relatively fast after cessation of the stressor, whereas neuroendocrine responses to stress were still present after 50 minutes [ 20 , 41 ]. Second, cognitive results also showed the predicted outcome: PM accuracy was within the normal range for PM studies (i.e., no ceiling effect occurred) and RTs and error rates were increased for PM REPEATED trials compared to standard trials, indicating aftereffects of completed intentions [ 3 , 4 ]. Most strikingly, however, despite a strong biological and subjective stress response and standard effects in the cognitive task, stress did not exert any influence on PM performance, ongoing-task performance, and aftereffects of completed intentions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The ability to remember to perform an intended action after some delay in the future is known as prospective memory (PM), which requires the retrieval of an intended action in the absence of a direct instruction, either at a pre-specified point of time (i.e., time-based PM) or to the appearance of an external mnemonic cue (i.e., PM cue; event-based PM) [ 1 ]. In addition to that, intention representations need to be deactivated once the intended action has been completed in order to prevent interference of completed intention representations with subsequent task performance [ 2 – 4 ]. Given continuous increase of work intensity in modern society, for example, high working speed and tight deadlines [ 5 ] with general increased numbers of workflow interruptions [ 6 ], it is surprising that the reliability of PM functioning in everyday life of healthy subjects has only recently attracted research in the scientific community, for example, with respect to mood states [ 7 ] or sleep disturbances [ 8 ].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To investigate the effect of finished or cancelled instructions, researchers compare reaction times to PM targets and matched control words. Longer reaction times for PM targets are interpreted as a failure to deactivate the related intention (Cohen, Dixon, & Lindsay, 2005;Scullin, Bugg, McDaniel, & Einstein, 2011;Walser et al, 2012;Walser, Plessow, Goschke, & Fischer, 2014). A further dependent variable is actual commission errors Bugg, Scullin, & McDaniel, 2013;Pink & Dodson, 2013;Scullin et al, 2012).…”
Section: Commission Errorsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This approach seemed preferable to comparing experiments by converting the average number of commission errors to the average proportion of commission errors, because some evidence suggests that the aftereffects of PM intentions decrease with repeated exposure to previously relevant PM targets(Walser, Plessow, Goschke, & Fischer, 2014). Note that the difference between the length of the finished PM phases in Experiment 1 versus Experiment 2 (and 3) was irrelevant to the analysis of the number of participants who made a commission error, because all participants who made an error in Experiment 1 did so within the first four trials.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%