1930
DOI: 10.1126/science.71.1847.536
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Tau Effect—an Example of Psychological Relativity

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

8
112
0

Year Published

1991
1991
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 122 publications
(120 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
8
112
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, the Tau and Kappa effects are two complementary illusions in which either stimulus timing presentation or spatial intervals between two sources lead to an illusion of distance or timing (Christopher Bill & Teft, 1972;Helson, 1930;Sarrazin et al, 2005). In experiments on the Tau effect, subjects systematically underestimated the spatial distance between stimuli separated by shorter temporal intervals (faster imputed velocities).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, the Tau and Kappa effects are two complementary illusions in which either stimulus timing presentation or spatial intervals between two sources lead to an illusion of distance or timing (Christopher Bill & Teft, 1972;Helson, 1930;Sarrazin et al, 2005). In experiments on the Tau effect, subjects systematically underestimated the spatial distance between stimuli separated by shorter temporal intervals (faster imputed velocities).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Especially in the field investigating non-veridical perceptions, crossmodal comparisons may be helpful to uncover underlying mechanisms (Christopher Bill & Teft, 1972;Helson, 1930;Sarrazin, Giraudo, & Pittenger, 2005). Evidence of a tactile RM would allow to disentangle body-centered and space-centered frames of references, which is relevant for patients with spatial processing deficits, in whom RM has previously been shown to be altered (Lenggenhager et al, 2012).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Distance judgements were found to vary as a function of the duration of the temporal ISIt. Since then, many studies -involving the visual (Bill and Teft 1969), auditory (Cohen et al 1953), or kinesthetic (Helson and King 1931;Lechelt and Bochert 1977) modality -have shown that when two constant spatial ISIs are associated with variable temporal ISIt, distance is overestimated or underestimated in accordance with the temporal ISIt, an effect Helson (1930) coined the "tau effect".…”
Section: The Issue Of Space-time Organizationmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…The present article addresses these fundamental questions that are of longstanding interest to experimental psychology. Indeed, the relation between spatial and temporal dimensions of stimuli was addressed in studies of perception in the first half of the last century (Benussi 1913;Helson 1930;Helson and King 1931), and, more recently, in the domain of motor control (Lee 2000). Links between spatial and temporal dynamics of action have been documented in the framework of precision aiming tasks (Fitts 1954;Meyer et al 1988;Mottet and Bootsma 1999) and bi-manual coordination tasks (Kelso 1981;Temprado et al 2001;Zanone and Kelso 1992).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…The above-described illusion, labeled tau effect (Helson, 1930;Helson & King, 1931), demonstrates the dependence of judgments of spatial distance on temporal duration: shorter temporal intervals are associated with shorter spatial distance judgments, and vice versa. The reverse phenomenon, i.e.…”
Section: Psychological Relativity Of Space and Timementioning
confidence: 99%