1993
DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb04188.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Transition to Reaching: Mapping Intention and Intrinsic Dynamics

Abstract: The onset of directed reaching demarks the emergence of a qualitatively new skill. In this study we asked how intentional reaching arises from infants' ongoing, intrinsic movement dynamics, and how first reaches become successively adapted to the task. We observed 4 infants weekly in a standard reaching task and identified the week of first arm-extended reach, and the 2 weeks before and after onset. The infants first reached at ages ranging from 12 to 22 weeks, and they used different strategies to get the toy… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

12
225
0
7

Year Published

1998
1998
2007
2007

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 281 publications
(244 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
12
225
0
7
Order By: Relevance
“…The youngest infants displayed a strong preference for trying to intercept the moving ball with both hands. It seems reasonable to suggest that this predominance of bimanual catching reflects the infants' intrinsic constraints (Corbetta & Thelen, 1996;Thelen et al, 1993). At this age infants show their first reaches, which are often but not always two handed, even when they reach for relatively small balls.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The youngest infants displayed a strong preference for trying to intercept the moving ball with both hands. It seems reasonable to suggest that this predominance of bimanual catching reflects the infants' intrinsic constraints (Corbetta & Thelen, 1996;Thelen et al, 1993). At this age infants show their first reaches, which are often but not always two handed, even when they reach for relatively small balls.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Because infants' goal-directed actions emerge from spontaneous movements (Thelen et al, 1993), the early coupling of visual attention and spontaneous movement could facilitate motor learning as the infant explores the linkages between his or her looking patterns and body movements. For example, infants may learn to modulate their movements by selectively attending to objects or people.…”
Section: Stability Of Couplingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, eye±head movements and, under some conditions, eye±hand movements are spatially and temporally linked in young infants (Ennouri & Bloch, 1996;van der Meer, van der Weel, & Lee, 1995;von Hofsten, 1984). Additionally, orienting in infants, often following the appearance of a new or unexpected visual stimulus, is characterized by an increase in visual inspection of the stimulus and a transient decrease in body movement (Sokolov, 1963;Stechler & Latz, 1966).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…According to Dynamic Systems Theory, patterns of behaviour, such as the typical smooth and straight trajectory of a reaching movement of an adult, act as 'attractors'. This means that a specific type of behaviour, which results from the effects of (1) its multiple component parts, such as muscle strength, body weight, postural support, the infant's mood, and brain development, and (2) the effect of environmental condition and task requirements, spontaneously adopts a specific organization [21][22][23] . Due to the properties of dynamic pattern formation, continuously occurring changes of the above mentioned component parts or changes of the environment or task can induce discontinuous changes (transitions) in behaviour.…”
Section: Dynamic Systems Theorymentioning
confidence: 99%