2007
DOI: 10.7771/1932-6246.1010
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Theory Driven Hints in the Cheap Necklace Problem: A Preliminary Investigation

Abstract: In this paper we conducted three experiments using the cheap necklace problem, which is regarded as an insight problem. The effects of two hints derived from two contemporary theoretical accounts of insight-Criterion for Satisfactory Progress theory (CSP) and Representational Change Theory (RCT)-were investigated. In Experiment 1, 78 participants made a single attempt at the problem, and significantly fewer participants given the CSP hint used an incorrect (maximizing) first move than participants given the RC… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
14
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 11 publications
0
14
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Among the earlier ideas are Luchins' concept of einstellung (blind spot) and Duncker's functional fixedness, as in Maher (1992). More recently, there have been two developed theoretical ideas: (1) Criterion for Satisfactory Progress theory (Chu, Dewald, & Chronicle, 2007;MacGregor, Ormerod, & Chronicle, 2001), and (2) Representational Change Theory (Knoblich, Ohlsson, Haider, & Rhenius, 1999). We will discuss these theories in more detail in Section 4.…”
Section: Barriers To a Theory Of Insight Problem Solvingmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Among the earlier ideas are Luchins' concept of einstellung (blind spot) and Duncker's functional fixedness, as in Maher (1992). More recently, there have been two developed theoretical ideas: (1) Criterion for Satisfactory Progress theory (Chu, Dewald, & Chronicle, 2007;MacGregor, Ormerod, & Chronicle, 2001), and (2) Representational Change Theory (Knoblich, Ohlsson, Haider, & Rhenius, 1999). We will discuss these theories in more detail in Section 4.…”
Section: Barriers To a Theory Of Insight Problem Solvingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…1 (Fall 2012) is given on how to represent the problem. This is the approach initiated originally by the Gestalt psychologists, as in Luchins (1942), Duncker (1945), Birch and Rabinowitz (1951), and it has continued to the present day as the common experimental paradigm for studying insight problem solving, as in MacGregor, Ormerod, and Chronicle (2001), Ormerod, MacGregor, and Chronicle (2002) and Chu, Dewald, and Chronicle (2007). Differences in the proportion of solvers may then be traced to the differences in the way that a problem is presented and therefore processed.…”
Section: Lack Of Many Experimental Paradigmsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…One can speculate that its smaller version, with just three 2-link chains and the total allowed cost of 10 cents, contains a good hint for solving the standard version: the very small number of reasonable alternatives in the former includes the right solution of breaking one of the given chains into individual links, which is harder to see for the larger version. In fact, it would be interesting to compare the usefulness of this hint with those used by Chu, Dewald, and Chronicle (2007) in their experiments with solving the Cheap Necklace problem by students.…”
Section: Classification Of Algorithmic Puzzles By Input Generalitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The Criterion for Satisfactory Progress (CSP) theory (Chu, Dewald, & Chronicle, 2007), formerly known as the Progress Monitoring Theory (MacGregor, Ormerod, & Chronicle, 2001), suggests that the difficulty with insight problems is that, initially, general problem-solving (GPS) heuristics are employed. Individuals appear to have a set when faced with difficult problems, that is, they perform the same incorrect sequence of moves over and over again before changing their mental representation of the problem.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%