1976
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1976.25-43
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

TITRATION OF SCHEDULE PARAMETERS BY PIGEONS1

Abstract: Pigeons were tested in a computer-controlled two-key chamber. A standard (nonchanging) schedule of reinforcement was in force on one key, and an adjusting schedule on the other. The schedules were available concurrently after each reinforcement, but after the first peck on either key (the choice peck), the schedule on the other key was made inoperative. The parameter of the adjusting schedule was decreased when the standard schedule was chosen and increased when the adjusting schedule was chosen. The standard … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
9
0

Year Published

1979
1979
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
8
1
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 86 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
1
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This process can account for maximization on concurrent VR schedules and for matching on concurrent VI schedules. In addition, it is consistent with a number of procedures explicitly designed to discriminate between melioration and global maximization (Boelens, 1984;Herrnstein & Vaughan, 1980;Vaughan, 1981;Vaughan, Kardish, & Wilson, 1982; also see Herrnstein & Heyman, 1979;Lea, 1976;Mazur, 1981).…”
Section: Tr Rrt2lsupporting
confidence: 70%
“…This process can account for maximization on concurrent VR schedules and for matching on concurrent VI schedules. In addition, it is consistent with a number of procedures explicitly designed to discriminate between melioration and global maximization (Boelens, 1984;Herrnstein & Vaughan, 1980;Vaughan, 1981;Vaughan, Kardish, & Wilson, 1982; also see Herrnstein & Heyman, 1979;Lea, 1976;Mazur, 1981).…”
Section: Tr Rrt2lsupporting
confidence: 70%
“…This situation is equivalent to an operantconditioning experiment in which the rate of return for responding to one alternative decreases with time, while the rate of return to be expected from another alternative (i.e., other patches, in the natural case) remains constant. An example might be a two-choice procedure: On one response key a ratio schedule where the size of the ratio increases with each food delivery and on the other a constant ratio schedule, the decreasing ratio would be reset to its initial value after each switch to the constant ratio (Hodos & Trumbule, 1967;Lea, 1976). Obviously, the optimal course is for the predator to leave a patch when the 'instantaneous rate of return drops below the rate of return from the habitat as a whole.…”
Section: Natural Feedback Functionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The last schedule described by Ferster and Skinner (1957) was an adjusting schedule in which the response requirement of a ratio schedule was increased or decreased after each reinforcer as a function of how long the animal paused before responding after each reinforcement. These adjusting procedures sometimes have been described as titration procedures (e.g., Lea, 1976;Weiss & Laties, 1959). Hackenberg and Axtell (1993) used an interactive dynamic reinforcement schedule to study the control of human behavior by longand short-term consequences.…”
Section: Interactive Dynamic Schedulesmentioning
confidence: 99%