2021
DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2021.105190
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Touchscreen-based assessment of food approach biases: Investigating reliability and item-specific preferences

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
29
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 25 publications
(31 citation statements)
references
References 68 publications
2
29
0
Order By: Relevance
“…On the other hand, when block order is counterbalanced, it can generate spurious interpersonal differences in approach-avoidance bias, suppressing correlational effects and creating spuriously high reliability. In fact, some studies suggest that participants respond faster due to training effects in the second block (Galler et al, 2022;Kahveci et al, 2021;Wittekind et al, 2021), while no order effects were found in other studies (Krieglmeyer & Deutsch, 2010;Rougier et al, 2018;van Alebeek et al, 2021).…”
Section: Aat Instructionsmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…On the other hand, when block order is counterbalanced, it can generate spurious interpersonal differences in approach-avoidance bias, suppressing correlational effects and creating spuriously high reliability. In fact, some studies suggest that participants respond faster due to training effects in the second block (Galler et al, 2022;Kahveci et al, 2021;Wittekind et al, 2021), while no order effects were found in other studies (Krieglmeyer & Deutsch, 2010;Rougier et al, 2018;van Alebeek et al, 2021).…”
Section: Aat Instructionsmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Cacioppo et al (1993), moreover, argued that: "subjects who have undergone food deprivation and who are exposed to edible ideographs […] may show stronger [approach tendencies] than un-deprived subjects" (see also Deutsch, 2005, 2015). Another indication of dynamic approach tendencies is that the AATs test-retest reliability is generally reported to be low, whereas its split-half reliability is generally reported to be high (Kahveci et. al., 2021;Machulska et al, 2022;Zech et al, 2022)-a pattern that indicates that a measure likely detects state changes (Hedge et al, 2018).…”
Section: Dynamic Food Approach Tendenciesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To test this hypothesis, we asked participants to report their subjective hunger and time since their last meal. In addition to testing these main hypotheses, we also explored the associations between food approach tendencies and food attractiveness as well as caloric density (food attractiveness has previously been shown to influence food approach tendencies; Kahveci et al, 2020;Kahveci et al 2021). To examine these associations, we varied the attractiveness and caloric density of the food stimuli used in the AAT.…”
Section: Experiments 1 (Pilot; Not Pre-registered)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Taken together, AAT investigations of approach biases towards food cues yielded rather mixed results. Most likely, this can (at least partly) be explained by methodological variations between studies in terms of response devices (keyboard, mouse, joystick, touchscreen), simulation of approach and avoidance movements (zoom or distance effects versus actual movements), and differences between task instructions in terms of relevant-feature versus irrelevant-feature AATs (see Kahveci et al, 2021;van Alebeek et al, 2021). In the context of 'wanting', one would assume an overall approach bias for palatable, HC food.…”
Section: Current State Of Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the context of 'wanting', one would assume an overall approach bias for palatable, HC food. However, recent research using an updated touchscreen, relevant-feature AAT points to an important role of individual momen-tary preferences and questions the widely held assumption of a general approach bias towards HC food (Kahveci et al, 2021).…”
Section: Current State Of Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%