Hedonic overconsumption (e.g., overconsumption of gratifying behaviors, e.g., eating, gaming, etc.) is common in daily life and often problematic, pointing to the need for adequate behavioral models. It has been theorized that when a consumption experience falls short of hedonic expectations people will generally consume more to compensate the shortfall. In this pre-registered experience sampling study we test this idea empirically via our hedonic compensation model, and we examine whether distraction during consumption triggers hedonic shortfall by compromising the consumption experience. Two-hundred and twenty participants received 49 identical surveys on their smartphone (seven per day for seven days), providing 6967 useable data-points. In each survey (if consumption was reported) participants indicated what (e.g., food, media) and how much they consumed, how much consumption conflicted with other personal goals, their consumption enjoyment and satisfaction, their post-consumption need for further gratification, and how distracted they were during consumption. Results support our hedonic compensation model: distraction during consumption predicted lower consumption enjoyment relative to expectations, which was followed by reduced consumption satisfaction, a greater post-consumption need for further gratification, a shorter duration to subsequent consumption, and more conflict between the subsequent consumption experience and other personal goals. The implications of these findings are discussed.
Researchers have suggested that overconsumption of food, alcohol, and drugs could be explained by chronically elevated approach tendencies to rewarding but unhealthy stimuli. Here, we use the example of food to show that dysregulated rather than chronically elevated approach tendencies are associated with negative health outcomes. To this end, we developed a new smartphone-based paradigm to measure dynamic changes in food approach tendencies outside the laboratory (piloted with N = 48). We demonstrated in three pre-registered experiments (total N = 367) that food approach tendencies decrease from before to after people have eaten. We further show that in overweight and obese participants, these dynamics are disrupted, as their food approach tendencies increase rather than decrease after meals. In addition to showing these effects based on traditional reaction time-based food approach tendencies, we also demonstrate these patterns in a novel measure of response force—a measure that has long been used to study motivation in animals but has received little attention in humans. Together, our findings suggest that both reaction-time based, and force-based approach tendencies change dynamically in accordance with people’s need states and that disruptions in these dynamics are associated with negative health outcomes, such as overweight and obesity.
Social welfare aims to support financially vulnerable households by protecting them from financial shocks and providing them with a basic standard of living. Many eligible households, however, do not take up social welfare. We present the results of in-depth interviews with 31 members of financially vulnerable households in two large Dutch cities about their experiences with welfare. We examined the role that money played in their lives, what inhibited them from taking up social welfare, and how they sought support. For many interviewed households, money was a source of stress. We found that the fear of reclaims and mistrust in government institutions were the main inhibitors for participating in welfare programs. Whereas the experience of shame and stigma were substantial inhibitors for claiming local welfare benefits, they were not for participating in national welfare programs. Both formal and informal help promoted welfare participation, but many participants lacked access to both forms of help. We discuss policies that could decrease the uncertainty associated with benefits receipt and give directions for future research.
We empirically test an integral model for healthcare and child support benefits take-up using a probability sample of the Dutch population (N = 905). To examine how different psychological factors, in conjunction, explain take-up, we apply model averaging with Akaike’s Information Criterion (AICC). For both types of benefits, people’s perceptions of eligibility best explain take-up. For healthcare benefits, take-up also relates to perceptions of need. Exploratory analyses suggest that for healthcare benefits but not for child support benefits, executive functions, self-efficacy, fear of reclaims, financial stress, and welfare stigma may indirectly explain take-up, with perceived eligibility as mediator. We find no support for knowledge, support, and administrative burden as explanatory factors in take-up. We discuss the results in relation to the Capability Opportunity Motivation Behavior (COM-B) model for developing behavioural change interventions.
Aims/hypothesis. Pharmacological conditioning, where an association is formed between a drug and a conditioned stimulus (CS, e.g., smell), can influence physiological processes. Evidence exists that it is possible to condition insulin effects in animals and healthy humans but it is unclear whether these effects can be observed in patients as well. The aim of this study was to investigate the effects of pharmacological conditioning with intranasal insulin on blood glucose, insulin, c-peptide, hunger and memory in patients with diabetes type-2 and age- and sex-matched healthy controls. Exploratively, we examined sex differences in these effects. We hypothesized that conditioning with insulin would trigger endogenous insulin release, decrease blood glucose and hunger, and improve memory. Methods. In a randomized double-blind controlled trial, thirty-two patients with diabetes type-2 (17 males, mean age=68.8, SD=11.86) and thirty-two healthy controls (17 males, mean age=67.8, SD=6.12) were randomly assigned to a conditioned or a control group. On day 1, participants in the conditioned group received 6 administrations of 20 units of intranasal insulin together with a CS (smell of rosewood oil) while the control group received a placebo with the CS. On day 2, participants in both groups received a placebo spray with the CS. Glucose, insulin and c-peptide were repeatedly measured in blood on both days. Hunger was measured with a self-report question, a mobile approach-avoidance task and a taste test. Memory was measured with an auditory verbal learning test.Results. Intranasal insulin prevented the drop of glucose levels in patients but not in healthy controls (B=0.03, SE=0.02, p=0.027), and decreased C-peptide levels in healthy controls but not patients (B=0.01, SE=0.001, p=0.008). Conditioned insulin effects also prevented the drop of blood glucose levels but only in male participants (for both healthy and patient groups) (B=0.001, SE=0.0003, p=0.024). Moreover, conditioning significantly decreased hunger in healthy participants (B=0.31, SE=0.09, p< 0.001). No effects of intranasal insulin or conditioning were found on insulin, approach-avoidance tendencies, calories eaten and memory. Conclusions/interpretation. Our study provides further evidence that conditioning with intranasal insulin might modify blood glucose levels and decrease hunger and that its effects differ depending on health status and sex. The unexpected direction of intranasal insulin effects on blood glucose might potentially be explained by age as our study population was older than participants from previous research. Conditioning with intranasal insulin might be beneficial for groups suffering from intensive hunger, but seems not be particularly suitable for blood glucose reduction. Diabetes type-2 patients might benefit from conditioning with glucose lowering drugs.
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2025 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.