2011
DOI: 10.1016/j.jcps.2010.12.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Unconscious transfer of meaning to brands

Abstract: We examine semantic conditioning in a consumer context. We subliminally paired neutral ideographs with attributes. In experiment 1, the ideographs served as primes during a lexical decision task and slowed down response times to target words with the opposite semantic meaning. In experiment 2, the ideographs served as brand names of beverages, and attitudinal responses to them were less favorable when the associated attributes were incongruent with existing schemas. These results showed that semantic condition… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
33
0
1

Year Published

2013
2013
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 42 publications
(36 citation statements)
references
References 33 publications
2
33
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…However, research investigating celebrity meaning transfer is relatively scarce. It only started a few years ago, resulting in a limited number of effect sizes that could be obtained (k = 9; Galli and Gorn 2011). Though the existing studies suggest strong transfer effects, further research is needed, also specifying boundary conditions (e.g., brand familiarity, *Miller and Allen 2012).…”
Section: Understudied Dependent Variablesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, research investigating celebrity meaning transfer is relatively scarce. It only started a few years ago, resulting in a limited number of effect sizes that could be obtained (k = 9; Galli and Gorn 2011). Though the existing studies suggest strong transfer effects, further research is needed, also specifying boundary conditions (e.g., brand familiarity, *Miller and Allen 2012).…”
Section: Understudied Dependent Variablesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Popular approaches to ensure information has been presented 'without awareness' include presenting critical information in a hidden format, for example by mixing target words in between filler items in scrambled sentence tasks or word search puzzles (e.g., Laran et al 2016;Tuk et al 2009) or by presenting information subliminally (e.g., Chartrand et al 2008;Dedonder et al, 2014;Fitzsimons et al 2008;Galli and Gorn 2011;Stahl et al 2016). …”
Section: Operationalization Via Experimental Manipulationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Hence, subliminal presentations should always be accompanied by sensitive measures of detection thresholds, so potentially aware participants can be excluded or the presentation times can be individually adjusted (Holender 1986). Such combined approaches are still rare in consumer research; we would like to highlight Galli and Gorn (2011) While a combined approach can in principle offer strong evidence for unconscious processes, it should be noted that failures to find evidence (i.e., null effects) for unconscious processes via subliminal presentations need to be interpreted with caution. We agree with WP and Evans (2014) that this might be a case where unfair standards of evidence are imposed on demonstrations of unconscious thought processes.…”
Section: Operationalization Via Experimental Manipulationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…that already possess valued associations in the hope that these associations will transfer to the new brand (Keller, 2003). In the context of brand alliances, research shows such transfers between partners do not appear to require in-depth deliberation (Galli and Gorn, 2011) and can result in positive (Park, Milberg and Lawson, 1991) or negative (Voltolato and Unnava, 2006) associations. Associative network models assume that partnerships between brands disproportionally affect the less-known brand since it is relatively empty of associations and is primed to receive them from an established brand (Levin and Levin, 2000, Washburn, Till and Priluck, 2000).…”
Section: Brand Alliancesmentioning
confidence: 99%