1985
DOI: 10.1177/001872088502700408
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Underlying Behavioral Parameters of the Operation of Touch-Input Devices: Biases, Models, and Feedback

Abstract: Touch-entry devices have exhibited a number of advantages in relatively low-resolution applications. Requirements for high-resolution input must, however, take into account the inherent biases and limitations of the operator. In this study performance biases in the use of an infrared touch-input device were examined with the input (display) surface at various declinations. A general bias to touch low and to the right of targets was found, the low bias increasing with increasing display declination. Attempts to… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

3
30
1

Year Published

1989
1989
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(34 citation statements)
references
References 7 publications
3
30
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Factors moderating accuracy and time of finger pointing on touch interfaces include: target size [9] and shape [5], the pointing device occluding the target's position and having larger size than the target (fat finger problem) [23], acquisition time limits, target location in relation to screen borders [5,32] and to other targets [24], finger orientation (roll, pitch, yaw) [23], posture of the user, e.g. sitting vs standing [16,34], the vertical [5] and horizontal [27] tilt of the screen, parallax [4], and feedback on whether a target was successfully touched [5].…”
Section: Touch Performancementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Factors moderating accuracy and time of finger pointing on touch interfaces include: target size [9] and shape [5], the pointing device occluding the target's position and having larger size than the target (fat finger problem) [23], acquisition time limits, target location in relation to screen borders [5,32] and to other targets [24], finger orientation (roll, pitch, yaw) [23], posture of the user, e.g. sitting vs standing [16,34], the vertical [5] and horizontal [27] tilt of the screen, parallax [4], and feedback on whether a target was successfully touched [5].…”
Section: Touch Performancementioning
confidence: 99%
“…sitting vs standing [16,34], the vertical [5] and horizontal [27] tilt of the screen, parallax [4], and feedback on whether a target was successfully touched [5].…”
Section: Touch Performancementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Much of the literature on TSDs points to parallax as a source of errors in operator inputs (see, for example, Beringer and Peterson, 1985;Beaton and Weiman, 1984;Logan, 1985;Stammers and Bird, 1980 the area of all touch locations that have a centroid which falls within the boundary of a display target (resulting in a hit) will vary depending upon the location of that display target. Figure 1 illustrates this relationship between touch resolution and hit area variation.…”
Section: Variables Influencing Human-tsd Interactionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To date, the only studies to be conducted with relatively high touch accuracy requirements have been done with fixed target locations (Beringer and Peterson, 1983;1985) and Visual feedback of touch registration has been shown to reduce touch errors and to lower training time requirements (Beringer and Peterson, 1985;Weiman et al, 1985) for TSO operators. Hopkin (1971) found that reducing TSO reliability to 99.…”
Section: Variables Influencing Human-tsd Interactionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some relevant studies determined the anthropometry for touchscreen workstations in order to increase their efficiency [1,13]; Schultz et al [16]. Schultz et al [16] reported that the optimal viewing angle for a touchscreen is between 19°and 54.5°off the horizontal.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%