2016
DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1134602
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Understanding underspecification: A comparison of two computational implementations

Abstract: Swets et al. (2008. Underspecification of syntactic ambiguities: Evidence from self-paced reading. Memory and Cognition, 36(1), 201–216) presented evidence that the so-called ambiguity advantage [Traxler et al. (1998). Adjunct attachment is not a form of lexical ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language, 39(4), 558–592], which has been explained in terms of the Unrestricted Race Model, can equally well be explained by assuming underspecification in ambiguous conditions driven by task-demands. Specif… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
18
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2025
2025

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 34 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
0
18
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We predicted that strongly activated visually derived representations should interfere with, and weakly active visually derived representations should facilitate, the processing of related semantic representations. The idea that competition can slow processing (in ambiguity resolution) receives support from the psycholinguistic literature on constraint-based models (e.g., Traxler et al, 1998 ; Vosse and Kempen, 2009 , see Arbib and Caplan, 1979 ; McClelland and Rumelhart, 1981 for related computational predecessors).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…We predicted that strongly activated visually derived representations should interfere with, and weakly active visually derived representations should facilitate, the processing of related semantic representations. The idea that competition can slow processing (in ambiguity resolution) receives support from the psycholinguistic literature on constraint-based models (e.g., Traxler et al, 1998 ; Vosse and Kempen, 2009 , see Arbib and Caplan, 1979 ; McClelland and Rumelhart, 1981 for related computational predecessors).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This measure includes not only fixations on the critical region but also regressive fixations on previous regions of the sentence. Finally, total reading time is the sum of all fixations in a given region (see, e.g., Liversedge et al, 1998 ; Rayner, 1998 ; Traxler et al, 1998 ; Rayner and Liversedge, 2004 ). All reading measures were calculated using the Data Viewer software (SR Research).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In closing, although it is clear that retrieval processes are not the only factor that determine processing difficulty in sentence comprehension [71,94,95,96,97,98,99,100], there Figure 5 . Dillon Expt 1: Ungrammatical agreement by participants.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…In an attempt to specify underspecification, Logačev and Vasishth's (2016) paper, entitled "Understanding underspecification: A comparison of two computational implementations", presents the results of a modelling experiment comparing partial specification and no-specification models of the parsing of globally ambiguous relative-clause attachments. The partial specification model was proposed by Swets, Desmet, Clifton, and Ferreira (2008).…”
Section: Contents Of This Special Issuementioning
confidence: 99%