2019
DOI: 10.1007/s11423-019-09681-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Validating theta power as an objective measure of cognitive load in educational video

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
34
1
1

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 53 publications
(40 citation statements)
references
References 57 publications
4
34
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Korbach et al (2020) provided an example of using eye‐tracking to investigate effects of pointing and tracing while learning from expository text and diagrams; while pointing and tracing was not associated with intrinsic and extraneous cognitive load self‐reports, eye‐tracking data revealed significant shifts in visual attention as well as deeper information processing. Kruger and Doherty (2016) have argued both intrinsic and extraneous cognitive load should be triangulated across “offline” self‐report measures collected alongside “online” pupillometric measures (e.g., averaged mean fixation duration, average fixation count, blink rate and blink latency) and electroencephalographic measures (e.g., alpha and theta power; see Castro‐Meneses, Kruger, & Doherty, 2020). Such studies may, however, present some interpretive challenges, as data from both pupillometry and electroencephalography are prone to movement artefacts that will need to be interpreted in the light of task‐relevant tracing actions as opposed to other task‐irrelevant movements.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Korbach et al (2020) provided an example of using eye‐tracking to investigate effects of pointing and tracing while learning from expository text and diagrams; while pointing and tracing was not associated with intrinsic and extraneous cognitive load self‐reports, eye‐tracking data revealed significant shifts in visual attention as well as deeper information processing. Kruger and Doherty (2016) have argued both intrinsic and extraneous cognitive load should be triangulated across “offline” self‐report measures collected alongside “online” pupillometric measures (e.g., averaged mean fixation duration, average fixation count, blink rate and blink latency) and electroencephalographic measures (e.g., alpha and theta power; see Castro‐Meneses, Kruger, & Doherty, 2020). Such studies may, however, present some interpretive challenges, as data from both pupillometry and electroencephalography are prone to movement artefacts that will need to be interpreted in the light of task‐relevant tracing actions as opposed to other task‐irrelevant movements.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…More generally, systematically varying the level of element interactivity of lesson materials in the same experiment would support investigation of the extent to which the tracing effect is moderated by this key cognitive load theory construct (Sweller, 2010). extraneous cognitive load should be triangulated across "offline" selfreport measures collected alongside "online" pupillometric measures (e.g., averaged mean fixation duration, average fixation count, blink rate and blink latency) and electroencephalographic measures (e.g., alpha and theta power; see Castro-Meneses, Kruger, & Doherty, 2020). Such studies may, however, present some interpretive challenges, as data from both pupillometry and electroencephalography are prone to movement artefacts that will need to be interpreted in the light of task-relevant tracing actions as opposed to other task-irrelevant movements.…”
Section: Test Phasementioning
confidence: 97%
“…Importantly, the link between theta power and memory and the link between alpha power and attention, are more evident in some regions than others. The link between theta oscillations and memory is most prominent in the frontal and central regions (Castro‐Meneses, Kruger, & Doherty, 2020; Wang, Antonenko, Keil, & Dawson, 2020), whereas the link between alpha oscillations and attention is most pronounced in the occipital and parietal regions (Jensen, Gelfand, Kounios, & Lisman, 2002; Whitmarsh, Oostenveld, Almeida, & Lundqvist, 2017).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There is no unique EEG feature that is directly related to cognitive load. Theta power has been suggested as an indicator for the average cognitive load of subjects and the linguistic complexity of educational videos (Castro-Meneses et al, 2019). Mu rhythm oscillations (8 − 13 Hz over the sensorimotor cortex) could be affected by the cognitive load during speech perception due to attention and working memory processes (Jenson et al, 2019).…”
Section: Eeg-pc Scores Related To Task Difficulty-based Cognitive Loadmentioning
confidence: 99%