“…Importantly, these psychologically meaningful parameters have allowed the DDM to answer questions of interest in the psychology, cognitive science, and neuroscience literature [8,11,9]. For example, the DDM has been used to better understand human economical decisions (e.g., gambling behavior [28], value-based decision making [29], and preference-based decision making [30,31]), applied decisions such as driving behavior [32,33,34], clinical questions (e.g., aging [35,36], autism spectrum disorder [37,38,39], attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [40,41], and dyslexia [42]), and theoretical questions such as performance optimality [43,44,45,46], just to name a few. While the standard DDM has been successful in capturing and explaining a wide range of behavioral patterns, several new variations of the model have been proposed based on ideas of neural plausibility, such as models with evidence leakage (e.g., the Ornstein-Uhlenbeck model [47]; see also [16,19]), boundaries that collapse over time (i.e., collapsing boundaries models [48,49]), and/or an urgency signal that amplifies the accumulated evidence with increasing time (i.e., urgency signal/gating models [19,17]).…”