1990
DOI: 10.1364/josaa.7.000128
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Visual interactions with luminance and chromatic stimuli

Abstract: The visibility of a 1 degree, 200-msec flash on a large yellow field was measured as a function of the intensity of a coincident pedestal flash (a flash that was the same in both temporal intervals of a two-alternative forced-choice trial). The various flashes were incremental (+Lum) or decremental (-Lum) yellow luminance flashes or green (+Chr) or red (-Chr) isoluminant chromatic flashes. With uncrossed conditions (Lum tests on Lum pedestals or Chr tests on Chr pedestals), we obtained the conventional dipper … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

12
48
0

Year Published

1993
1993
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 131 publications
(60 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
12
48
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus the grating can be detected with high probability (by RG) before a significant motion response is generated (by SPO). Cole et al (1990) also obtained an exponent of -2 for detection of a chromatic flash. The exponent for motion of this chromatic pattern is considerably higher than for detection.…”
Section: Moving Gratings: Detection and Direction Identificationmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…Thus the grating can be detected with high probability (by RG) before a significant motion response is generated (by SPO). Cole et al (1990) also obtained an exponent of -2 for detection of a chromatic flash. The exponent for motion of this chromatic pattern is considerably higher than for detection.…”
Section: Moving Gratings: Detection and Direction Identificationmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…It has been used extensively in the spatial domain to investigate the response characteristics of channels or mechanisms that are differentially sensitive to spatial frequency and orientation (e.g., Bird, Henning, & Wichmann, 2002;Henning & Wichmann, 2007;Nachmias & Sansbury, 1974;Wichmann, 1999;Yang & Makous, 1995). But it has also been used to investigate the properties of, for example, color and luminance mechanisms (e.g., Chen, Foley, & Brainard, 2000;Cole, Stromeyer, & Kronauer, 1990;Mullen & Losada, 1994;Switkes, Bradley, & De Valois, 1988); and ON-and OFF-channels (e.g., Bowen, 1995). The effect has also been reported with flickering or drifting gratings (e.g., Anderson & Vingrys, 2001;Boynton & Foley, 1999;Stromeyer, Kronauer, & Madsen, 1984) and with uniform flickering targets (Anderson & Vingrys, 2000;Stockman & MacLeod, 1985).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Empirical evidence in support of the primacy of luminance comes from studies showing impaired perception of figural boundaries at isoluminance, whether contours are real (Eskew & Boynton, 1987) or illusory (Livingstone & Hubel, 1987, 1988. Furthermore, chromatic discrimination is disrupted at isoluminance (Boynton, Hayhoe, & MacLeod, 1977) and enhanced by luminance pedestals (Cole, Stromeyer, & Kronauer, 1990), findings that suggest that chromatic processing is perceptually dependent on discontinuities in luminance. Cavanagh (1985Cavanagh ( , 1987, among others (McIlhagga & Mullen, 1996;Ramachandran, 1992;Ware & Cowan, 1983), has been critical of Gregory's (1977Gregory's ( , 1979 position, arguing instead that two-dimensional figure-ground perception is accomplished as easily by color or textural differences as by luminance differences.…”
Section: Privileged Statusmentioning
confidence: 99%