1983
DOI: 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90036-7
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Wernicke's aphasia and normal language processing: A case study in cognitive neuropsychology

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
70
0

Year Published

1991
1991
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 164 publications
(70 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
0
70
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We have reported the case of a patient with Wernicke's aphasia who suffered from severe neologistic jargon (for reviews and related cases, see Butterworth, 1979Butterworth, , 1992Ellis, Miller, & Sin, 1983;Lecours, 1982). The neologisms, although generally close to the target in terms of overall syllabic structure, were marred by pervasive phoneme substitutions.…”
Section: General Discussion Summarymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We have reported the case of a patient with Wernicke's aphasia who suffered from severe neologistic jargon (for reviews and related cases, see Butterworth, 1979Butterworth, , 1992Ellis, Miller, & Sin, 1983;Lecours, 1982). The neologisms, although generally close to the target in terms of overall syllabic structure, were marred by pervasive phoneme substitutions.…”
Section: General Discussion Summarymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The evidence we review shows that writing is not dependent on phonological processes.'! One type of evidence that has been cited in support of the autonomy of writing processes from phonology is the dissociation of writing and speaking impairments (e.g., Assai, Buttet, & Jolivet, 1981;Blanken, de Langen, Dittmann, & Wallesch, 1989;Bub & Kertesz, 1982;Caramazza, Berndt, & Basili, 1983;Ellis, Miller, & Sin, 1983;Hier & Mohr, 1977;Kremin, 1987;Patterson & Shewell, 1987). However, these reports did not explore the deficits in enough detail to resolve issues concerning the influence of both sublexical and lexical phonological processing on writing.…”
Section: The Independence Of Phonological and Orthographic Output Repmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, cases in which difficulties in oral responding result from motor difficulties affecting the articulators would be irrelevant. Anumber of cases exhibiting superior written vs. spoken naming with intact articulatory abilities have, in fact, been reported across a number of languages (Assal, Buttet, & Jolivet, 1981;Basso et al, 1978;Bub & Kertesz, 1982;Caramazza, Berndt, & Basili, 1983;Caramazza & Hillis, 1990;Coslett, Gonzalez-Rothi, & Heilman, 1984;Ellis, Miller, & Sin, 1983; Friederici, Schoenle, & Goodglass, 1981; Grashey, 1885;Hier & Mohr, 1977;Lecours & Rouillon, 1976;Leischner, 1969;Levine, Calvanio, & Popovics, 1982;Lhermitte & Dér ouesné, 1974;Lichtheim, 1885;Mohr, Sidman, Stoddart, Leicester, & Morton, 1980;Mohr, Pessin, Finkelstein, Funkenstein, Duncan, &Davis, 1978;Nickels, 1992;Patterson & Shewell, 1987;Rapp & Caramazza, 1997;Semenza, Cipolotti, & Denes, 1992). However, although it is obviously necessary to rule out a peripheral articulatory source of the spoken errors, the finding of intact articulation is also insufficient.…”
Section: Establishing the Locus Of Impairment: Lexical Vs Post-lexicalmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There are anumberof individuals whoexhibit fluent, well-articulated speech characterised by neologisms and phonemic errors in the context of relatively spared written performance (Assal et al, 1981;Caramazza et al, 1983;Coslett et al, 1984;Ellis et al, 1983;Lhermitte & Dér ouesné, 1974;Patterson & Shewell, 1987;Rapp & Caramazza, in press;Semenza et al, 1992). For example, one of the individuals described by Lhermitte and Déro uesné (1974) was 74% correct in written production but only 8% correct in oral production.…”
Section: Phonemic Errors and Spoken Neologisms + Relatively Good Writmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation