2011
DOI: 10.1177/0361684310383811
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

What We “Say” and What We “Think” About Female Managers

Abstract: In two studies, we investigated implicit gender stereotypes of successful managers. Using an adaptation of the Implicit Association Test (IAT) named the Successful Manager IAT (SM-IAT) in Study 1, we found that male participants were more likely to implicitly associate men with successful manager traits and women with unsuccessful manager traits compared to reversed pairings. Women, individuals high in internal motivation to respond without sexism, and those low in external motivation to respond without sexism… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

0
23
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 41 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 49 publications
0
23
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The informed faking of low scores might be indicated by effects on IAT a , but the naïve and informed faking of high scores might not be indicated, because faking high scores only affects the construct-related IAT effect (i.e., IAT v ). From a practical standpoint, faking low is indeed the more likely faking condition, because low scores often represent the associations that are not socially stigmatized in particularly sensitive areas, such as pedophilia, racism, stereotypes, or sexism (e.g., Agerström & Rooth, 2011;Banse, Schmidt, & Clarbour, 2010;Banse et al, 2001;Carlsson & Björklund, 2010;Gray, Brown, MacCulloch, Smith, & Snowden, 2005;Greenwald & Banaji, 1995;Greenwald et al, 1998;Latu et al, 2011).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The informed faking of low scores might be indicated by effects on IAT a , but the naïve and informed faking of high scores might not be indicated, because faking high scores only affects the construct-related IAT effect (i.e., IAT v ). From a practical standpoint, faking low is indeed the more likely faking condition, because low scores often represent the associations that are not socially stigmatized in particularly sensitive areas, such as pedophilia, racism, stereotypes, or sexism (e.g., Agerström & Rooth, 2011;Banse, Schmidt, & Clarbour, 2010;Banse et al, 2001;Carlsson & Björklund, 2010;Gray, Brown, MacCulloch, Smith, & Snowden, 2005;Greenwald & Banaji, 1995;Greenwald et al, 1998;Latu et al, 2011).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…
AbstractAlthough explicit stereotypes of women in the workplace have become increasingly positive (Duehr & Bono, 2006), negative stereotypes persist at an implicit level, with women being more likely associated with incompetent-and men with competent-managerial traits (Latu et al, 2011). Drawing upon work on self-fulfilling prophecies and interracial interactions, we investigated whether and how implicit and explicit gender stereotypes held by both male interviewers and female applicants predicted women's interview outcomes.
…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, organizations are increasingly committed to promoting gender equality through diversity programs and policies. However, despite an improvement in explicit gender stereotypes (Duehr & Bono, 2006;Stoker, Van der Velde, & Lammers, 2012), negative stereotypes persist at an implicit level, with women being more likely to be implicitly associated with negative managerial traits (Latu et al, 2011) and with roles incompatible with leadership (Nosek, Banaji, & Greenwald, 2002;Rudman & Kilianski, 2000).One pivotal point at which gender stereotypes can manifest themselves in workplace inequality is in the door to employment itself: the job interview. But what is the role that gender stereotypes play in female job applicants' interview performance and employers' interview-based job decisions?…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations