“…Hence, records in other languages, commentaries, narrative/qualitative reviews, editorials, book chapters, and abstracts were not considered for further analysis. The following exclusion criteria were also applied: (a) studies that manipulated the cognitive load of the PM cue (e.g., Ballhausen et al, 2017 ; Cohen, 2013 ), as these conditions have been shown to affect OT performance ( Meier & Zimmermann, 2015 ), (b) studies that included delay-execute conditions or activity-based PM tasks (i.e., the PM response had to be performed after a particular task has finished; Brewer et al, 2011 ), as PM cues did not appear during the OT, (c) studies that included clinical samples, as PM might be particularly affected in this context (e.g., Albinski et al, 2012 ), (d) studies that involved drug interventions and/or ingestion of substances (e.g., Rusted, & Trawley, 2006 ), or that manipulated other factors including sleep (e.g., Barner et al, 2016 ), or that used neuromodulation techniques such as transcranial magnetic stimulation (e.g., Basso et al, 2010 ), (e) experiments that included children, adolescents, and older adults (e.g., Cheie et al, 2017 ; Zollig et al, 2007 ) given that previous research had demonstrated that PM follows an inverted U-shape developmental trajectory ( Zuber & Kliegel, 2019 ; Zimmermann & Meier, 2006 ). So, by including only young and middle-age adults, age effects were somewhat restricted to this developmental stage.…”