2019
DOI: 10.1111/ejn.14503
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

When is irony influenced by communicative constraints? ERP evidence supporting interactive models

Abstract: Distinct theoretical proposals have described how communicative constraints (contextual biases, speaker identity) impact verbal irony processing. Modular models assume that social and contextual factors have an effect at a late stage of processing. Interactive models claim that contextual biases are considered early on. The constraint‐satisfaction model further assumes that speaker's and context's characteristics can compete at early stages of analysis. The present ERP study teased apart these models by testin… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 24 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 57 publications
(134 reference statements)
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The result was similar to a two-stage processing pattern, and the parallel Constraint-Satisfaction Model might be taken into more consideration. In some ERP studies, the comprehension of irony might involve N400- or P600-like (late positivity) effects, where the N400 effect was usually interpreted as the semantic integration between context and literal meaning, while P600 might refer to the pragmatic inference of the ironic intent ( Cornejo et al, 2007 ; Regel et al, 2011 ; Spotorno et al, 2013 ; Filik et al, 2014 ; Caffarra et al, 2019 ; Mauchand et al, 2021 ). Despite the fact that the literality and the social status effect occurred in different regions in the current study, the time course of literality and status processing was similar in principle to the N400 and P600 effects found in previous studies.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The result was similar to a two-stage processing pattern, and the parallel Constraint-Satisfaction Model might be taken into more consideration. In some ERP studies, the comprehension of irony might involve N400- or P600-like (late positivity) effects, where the N400 effect was usually interpreted as the semantic integration between context and literal meaning, while P600 might refer to the pragmatic inference of the ironic intent ( Cornejo et al, 2007 ; Regel et al, 2011 ; Spotorno et al, 2013 ; Filik et al, 2014 ; Caffarra et al, 2019 ; Mauchand et al, 2021 ). Despite the fact that the literality and the social status effect occurred in different regions in the current study, the time course of literality and status processing was similar in principle to the N400 and P600 effects found in previous studies.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Rather, the immediate on-line uptake and structuring of prosodic cues (especially sarcastic prosody) can point directly to nonliteral meanings, allowing listeners to predict and constrain a representation of the speaker's ironic intent as a "best fit" solution (Gibbs, 2002). This process is best captured by the parallelconstraint satisfaction model (Katz et al, 2004;Pexman, 2008), which is receiving increasing support (Caffarra et al, 2019;Deliens et al, 2018;Kowatch et al, 2013;Spotorno et al, 2013). When prosodic cues are salient and continuously processed, initial representations of ironic intent appear to facilitate linguistic processing which does not require pragmatic reinterpretation, although these operations are somewhat altered for less prototypical, negative statements communicating irony.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Saying "You're such a lousy driver" to a friend who is clearly very good implies an intent to playfully compliment, not to criticize. Despite similarities in structure, the intent of ironic compliments does not seem to be recognized as accurately as ironic criticisms or processed in the same manner (Bruntsch & Ruch, 2017;Caillies et al, 2019;Caffarra et al, 2019). This may be due to the "asymmetry of affect" (Matthews, Hancock, & Dunham, 2006;Pexman & Olineck, 2002).…”
Section: Verbal Irony and Prosodymentioning
confidence: 97%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…But it seems unlikely that replication efforts will somehow clean up this catalog of experimental findings to reveal a simple, comprehensive set of data which clearly points to one theoretical model of irony understanding that can be applied to all people in all situations of verbal irony use. Nonetheless, the various, sometimes complex patterns of experimental results may highlight different systems of constraint that flexibly operate to produce relevant irony interpretations in different task-specific and people-specific contexts (e.g., constraint-satisfaction models, see Campbell and Katz, 2012;Caffarra et al, 2019).…”
Section: A Case Study Examplementioning
confidence: 99%